Title: The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
Subject:
Author: Charles Darwin
Keywords:
Creator:
PDF Version: 1.2
Page No 1
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
Charles Darwin
Page No 2
Table of Contents
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals............................................................................................1
Charles Darwin........................................................................................................................................1
INTRODUCTION...................................................................................................................................1
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION...............................................................11
CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_continued_. .....................................21
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. ...................................25
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS................................................................32
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS..................................................................44
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING....................55
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. ................................67
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION.........................75
CHAPTER IX.
REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION...............85
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. ..............................................................................................91
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE,
ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION............................98
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. ........................................108
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING......119
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY...................................................135
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
i
Page No 3
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
Charles Darwin
INTRODUCTION
CHAP. IGENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION.
CHAP. IIGENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_continued_.
CHAP. IIIGENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_.
CHAP. IVMEANS OF EXPRESSION. IN ANIMALS.
CHAP. V.SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS.
CHAP. VI.SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING.
CHAP. VII.LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR.
CHAP. VIII.JOY, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION.
CHAP. IX.REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESS DETERMINATION.
CHAP. X.HATRED AND ANGER.
CHAP. XI.DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.
CHAP. XII.SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR.
CHAP. XIII.SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING.
CHAP. XIV.CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY.
INTRODUCTION.
MANY works have been written on Expression, but a greater number on Physiognomy,that is, on the
recognition of character through the study of the permanent form of the features. With this latter subject I am
not here concerned. The older treatises,[1] which I have consulted, have been of little or no service to me.
The famous `Conferences'[2] of the painter Le Brun, published in 1667, is the best known ancient work, and
contains some good remarks. Another somewhat old essay, namely, the `Discours,' delivered 17741782, by
the wellknown Dutch anatomist Camper,[3] can hardly be considered as having made any marked advance
in the subject. The following works, on the contrary, deserve the fullest consideration.
Sir Charles Bell, so illustrious for his discoveries in physiology, published in 1806 the first edition, and in
[1] J. Parsons, in his paper in the Appendix to the `Philosophical Transactions' for 1746, p. 41, gives a list of
fortyone old authors who have written on Expression.
[2] Conferences sur l'expression des differents Caracteres des Passions.' Paris, 4to, 1667. I always quote from
the republication of the `Conferences' in the edition of Lavater, by Moreau, which appeared in 1820, as given
in vol. ix. p. 257.
[3] `Discours par Pierre Camper sur le moyen de representer les diverses passions,' 1792. 1844 the third
edition of his `Anatomy and Philosophy of Expression.'[4] He may with justice be said, not only to have laid
the foundations of the subject as a branch of science, but to have built up a noble structure. His work is in
every way deeply interesting; it includes graphic descriptions of the various emotions, and is admirably
illustrated. It is generally admitted that his service consists chiefly in having shown the intimate relation
which exists between the movements of expression and those of respiration. One of the most important
points, small as it may at first appear, is that the muscles round the eyes are involuntarily contracted during
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 1
Page No 4
violent expiratory efforts, in order to protect these delicate organs from the pressure of the blood. This fact,
which has been fully investigated for me with the greatest kindness by Professors Donders of Utrecht, throws,
as we shall hereafter see, a flood of light on several of the most important expressions of the human
countenance. The merits of Sir C. Bell's work have been undervalued or quite ignored by several foreign
writers, but have been fully admitted by some, for instance by M. Lemoine,[5] who with great justice
says:"Le livre de Ch. Bell devrait etre medite par quiconque essaye de faire parler le visage de l'homme,
par les philosophes aussi bien que par les artistes, car, sous une apparence plus legere et sous le pretexte de
l'esthetique, c'est un des plus beaux monuments de la science des rapports du physique et du moral."
[4] I always quote from the third edition, 1844, which was published after the death of Sir C. Bell, and
contains his latest corrections. The first edition of 1806 is much inferior in merit, and does not include some
of his more important views.
[5] `De la Physionomie et de la Parole,' par Albert Lemoine, 1865, p. 101.
From reasons which will presently be assigned, Sir C. Bell did not attempt to follow out his views as far as
they might have been carried. He does not try to explain why different muscles are brought into action under
different emotions; why, for instance, the inner ends of the eyebrows are raised, and the corners of the mouth
depressed, by a person suffering from grief or anxiety.
In 1807 M. Moreau edited an edition of Lavater on Physiognomy,[6] in which he incorporated several of his
own essays, containing excellent descriptions of the movements of the facial muscles, together with many
valuable remarks. He throws, however, very little light on the philosophy of the subject. For instance, M.
Moreau, in speaking of the act of frowning, that is, of the contraction of the muscle called by French writers
the _soucilier_ (_corrigator supercilii_), remarks with truth:"Cette action des sourciliers est un des
symptomes les plus tranches de l'expression des affections penibles ou concentrees." He then adds that these
muscles, from their attachment and position, are fitted "a resserrer, a concentrer les principaux traits de la
_face_, comme il convient dans toutes ces passions vraiment oppressives ou profondes, dans ces affections
dont le sentiment semble porter l'organisation a revenir sur ellememe, a se contracter et a _s'amoindrir_,
comme pour offrir moins de prise et de surface a des impressions redoutables ou importunes." He who thinks
that remarks of this kind throw any light on the meaning or origin of the different expressions, takes a very
different view of the subject to what I do.
[6] `L'Art de connaitre les Hommes,' par G. Lavater. The earliest edition of this work, referred to in the
preface to the edition of 1820 in ten volumes, as containing the observations of M. Moreau, is said to have
been published in 1807; and I have no doubt that this is correct, because the `Notice sur Lavater' at the
commencement of volume i. is dated April 13, 1806. In some bibliographical works, however, the date of
18051809 is given, but it seems impossible that 1805 can be correct. Dr. Duchenne remarks (`Mecanisme
de la Physionomie Humaine,'8vo edit. 1862, p. 5, and `Archives Generales de Medecine,' Jan. et Fev. 1862)
that M. Moreau "_a compose pour son ouvrage un article important_," in the year 1805; and I find in volume
i. of the edition of 1820 passages bearing the dates of December 12, 1805, and another January 5, 1806,
besides that of April 13, 1806, above referred to. In consequence of some of these passages having thus been
COMPOSED in 1805, Dr. Duchenne assigns to M. Moreau the priority over Sir C. Bell, whose work, as we
have seen, was published in 1806. This is a very unusual manner of determining the priority of scientific
works; but such questions are of extremely little importance in comparison with their relative merits. The
passages above quoted from M. Moreau and from Le Brun are taken in this and all other cases from the
edition of 1820 of Lavater, tom. iv. p. 228, and tom. ix. p. 279. " In the above passage there is but a slight, if
any, advance in the philosophy of the subject, beyond that reached by the painter Le Brun, who, in 1667, in
describing the expression of fright, says:"Le sourcil qui est abaisse d'un cote et eleve de l'autre, fait voir
que la partie elevee semble le vouloir joindre au cerveau pour le garantir du mal que l'ame apercoit, et le cote
qui est abaisse et qui parait enfle, nous fait trouver dans cet etat par les esprits qui viennent du cerveau en
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 2
Page No 5
abondance, comme polir couvrir l'aine et la defendre du mal qu'elle craint; la bouche fort ouverte fait voir le
saisissement du coeur, par le sang qui se retire vers lui, ce qui l'oblige, voulant respirer, a faire un effort qui
est cause que la bouche s'ouvre extremement, et qui, lorsqu'il passe par les organes de la voix, forme un son
qui n'est point articule; que si les muscles et les veines paraissent enfles, ce n'est que par les esprits que le
cerveau envoie en ces partiesla." I have thought the foregoing sentences worth quoting, as specimens of the
surprising nonsense which has been written on the subject.
`The Physiology or Mechanism of Blushing,' by Dr. Burgess, appeared in 1839, and to this work I shall
frequently refer in my thirteenth Chapter.
In 1862 Dr. Duchenne published two editions, in folio and octavo, of his `Mecanisme de la Physionomie
Humaine,' in which he analyses by means of electricity, and illustrates by magnificent photographs, the
movements of the facial muscles. He has generously permitted me to copy as many of his photographs as I
desired. His works have been spoken lightly of, or quite passed over, by some of his countrymen. It is
possible that Dr. Duchenne may have exaggerated the importance of the contraction of single muscles in
giving expression; for, owing to the intimate manner in which the muscles are connected, as may be seen in
Henle's anatomical drawings[7]the best I believe ever published it is difficult to believe in their separate
action. Nevertheless, it is manifest that Dr. Duchenne clearly apprehended this and other sources of error, and
as it is known that he was eminently successful in elucidating the physiology of the muscles of the hand by
the aid of electricity, it is probable that he is generally in the right about the muscles of the face. In my
opinion, Dr. Duchenne has greatly advanced the subject by his treatment of it. No one has more carefully
studied the contraction of each separate muscle, and the consequent furrows produced on the skin. He has
also, and this is a very important service, shown which muscles are least under the separate control of the
will. He enters very little into theoretical considerations, and seldom attempts to explain why certain muscles
and not others contract under the influence of certain emotions. A distinguished French anatomist, Pierre
Gratiolet, gave a course of lectures on Expression at the Sorbonne, and his notes were published (1865) after
his death, under the title of `De la Physionomie et des Mouvements d'Expression.' This is a very interesting
work, full of valuable observations. His theory is rather complex, and, as far as it can be given in a single
sentence (p. 65), is as follows:"Il resulte, de tous les faits que j'ai rappeles, que les sens, l'imagination et la
pensee ellememe, si elevee, si abstraite qu'on la suppose, ne peuvent s'exercer sans eveiller un sentiment
correlatif, et que ce sentiment se traduit directement, sympathiquement, symboliquement ou
metaphoriquement, dans toutes les spheres des organs exterieurs, qui la racontent tous, suivant leur mode
d'action propre, comme si chacun d'eux avait ete directement affecte."
[7] `Handbuch der Systematischen Anatomie des Menschen.' Band I. Dritte Abtheilung, 1858.
Gratiolet appears to overlook inherited habit, and even to some extent habit in the individual; and therefore he
fails, as it seems to me, to give the right explanation, or any explanation at all, of many gestures and
expressions. As an illustration of what he calls symbolic movements, I will quote his remarks (p. 37), taken
from M. Chevreul, on a man playing at billiards. "Si une bille devie legerement de la direction que le joueur
pretend zlui imprimer, ne l'avezvous pas vu cent fois la pousser du regard, de la tete et meme des epaules,
comme si ces mouvements, purement symboliques, pouvaient rectifier son trajet? Des mouvements non
moins significatifs se produisent quand la bille manque d'une impulsion suffisante. Et cliez les joueurs
novices, ils sont quelquefois accuses au point d'eveiller le sourire sur les levres des spectateurs." Such
movements, as it appeirs to me, may be attributed simply to habit. As often as a man has wished to move an
object to one side, he has always pushed it to that side when forwards, he has pushed it forwards; and if he
has wished to arrest it, he has pulled backwards. Therefore, when a man sees his ball travelling in a wrong
direction, and he intensely wishes it to go in another direction, he cannot avoid, from long habit,
unconsciously performing movements which in other cases he has found effectual.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 3
Page No 6
As an instance of sympathetic movements Gratiolet gives (p. 212) the following case:"un jeune chien A
oreilles droites, auquel son maitre presente de loin quelque viande appetissante, fixe avec ardeur ses yeux sur
cet objet dont il suit tous les mouvements, et pendant que les yeux regardent, les deux oreilles se portent en
avant comme si cet objet pouvait etre entendu." Here, instead of speaking of sympathy between the ears and
eyes, it appears to me more simple to believe, that as dogs during many generations have, whilst intently
looking at any object, pricked their ears in order to perceive any sound; and conversely have looked intently
in the direction of a sound to which they may have listened, the movements of these organs have become
firmly associated together through longcontinued habit.
Dr. Piderit published in 1859 an essay on Expression, which I have not seen, but in which, as he states, he
forestalled Gratiolet in many of his views. In 1867 he published his `Wissenschaftliches System der Mimik
und Physiognomik.' It is hardly possible to give in a few sentences a fair notion of his views; perhaps the two
following sentences will tell as much as can be briefly told: "the muscular movements of expression are in
part related to imaginary objects, and in part to imaginary sensorial impressions. In this proposition lies the
key to the comprehension of all expressive muscular movements." (s. 25) Again, "Expressive movements
manifest themselves chiefly in the numerous and mobile muscles of the face, partly because the nerves by
which they are set into motion originate in the most immediate vicinity of the mindorgan, but partly also
because these muscles serve to support the organs of sense." (s. 26.) If Dr. Piderit had studied Sir C. Bell's
work, he would probably not have said (s. 101) that violent laughter causes a frown from partaking of the
nature of pain; or that with infants (s. 103) the tears irritate the eyes, and thus excite the contraction of the
surrounding in muscles. Many good remarks are scattered throughout this volume, to which I shall hereafter
refer.
Short discussions on Expression may be found in various works, which need not here be particularised. Mr.
Bain, however, in two of his works has treated the subject at some length. He says,[8] "I look upon the
expression socalled as part and parcel of the feeling. I believe it to be a general law of the mind that along
with the fact of inward feeling or consciousness, there is a diffusive action or excitement over the bodily
members." In another place he adds, "A very considerable number of the facts may be brought under the
following principle: namely, that states of pleasure are connected with an increase, and states of pain with an
abatement, of some, or all, of the vital functions." But the above law of the diffusive action of feelings seems
too general to throw much light on special expressions.
Mr. Herbert Spencer, in treating of the Feelings in his `Principles of Psychology' (1855), makes the following
remarks:"Fear, when strong, expresses itself in cries, in efforts to hide or escape, in palpitations and
tremblings; and these are just the manifestations that would accompany an actual experience of the evil
feared. The destructive passions are shown in a general tension of the muscular system, in gnashing of the
teeth and protrusion of the claws, in dilated eyes and nostrils in growls; and these are weaker forms of the
actions that accompany the killing of prey." Here we have, as I believe, the true theory of a large number of
expressions; but the chief interest and difficulty of the subject lies in following out the wonderfully complex
results. I infer that some one (but who he is I have not been able to ascertain) formerly advanced a nearly
similar view, for Sir C. Bell says,[9] "It has been maintained that what are called the external signs of
passion, are only the concomitants of those voluntary movements which the structure renders necessary." Mr.
Spencer has also published[10] a valuable essay on the physiology of Laughter, in which he insists on "the
general law that feeling passing a certain pitch, habitually vents itself in bodily action," and that "an overflow
of nerveforce undirected by any motive, will manifestly take first the most habitual routes; and if these do
not suffice, will next overflow into the less habitual ones." This law I believe to be of the highest importance
in throwing light on our subject.`
[8] `The Senses and the Intellect,' 2nd edit. 1864, pp. 96 and 288. The preface to the first edition of this work
is dated June, 1855. See also the 2nd edition of Mr. Bain's work on the `Emotions and Will.'
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 4
Page No 7
[9] `The Anatomy of Expression,' 3rd edit. p. 121.
[10] `Essays, Scientific, Political, and Speculative,' Second Series, 1863, p. 111. There is a discussion on
Laughter in the First Series of Essays, which discussion seems to me of very inferior value.
[11] Since the publication of the essay just referred to, Mr. Spencer has written another, on "Morals and
Moral Sentiments," in the `Fortnightly Review,' April 1, 1871, p. 426. He has, also, now published his final
conclusions in vol. ii. of the second edit. of the `Principles of Psychology,' 1872, p. 539. I may state, in order
that I may not be accused of trespassing on Mr. Spencer's domain, that I announced in my `Descent of Man,'
that I had then written a part of the present volume: my first MS. notes on the subject of expression bear the
date of the year 1838.
All the authors who have written on Expression, with the exception of Mr. Spencerthe great expounder of
the principle of Evolution appear to have been firmly convinced that species, man of course included,
came into existence in their present condition. Sir C. Bell, being thus convinced, maintains that many of our
facial muscles are "purely instrumental in expression;" or are "a special provision" for this sole object.[12]
But the simple fact that the anthropoid apes possess the same facial muscles as we do,[13] renders it very
improbable that these muscles in our case serve exclusively for expression; for no one, I presume, would be
inclined to admit that monkeys have been endowed with special muscles solely for exhibiting their hideous
grimaces. Distinct uses, independently of expression, can indeed be assigned with much probability for
almost all the facial muscles.
Sir C. Bell evidently wished to draw as broad a distinction as possible between man and the lower animals;
and he consequently asserts that with "the lower creatures there is no expression but what may be referred,
more or less plainly, to their acts of volition or necessary instincts." He further maintains that their faces
"seem chiefly capable of expressing rage and fear."[14] But man himself cannot express love and humility by
external signs, so plainly as does a dog, when with drooping ears, hanging lips, flexuous body, and wagging
tail, he meets his beloved master. Nor can these movements in the dog be explained by acts of volition or
necessary instincts, any more than the beaming eyes and smiling cheeks of a man when he meets an old
friend. If Sir C. Bell had been questioned about the expression of affection in the dog, he would no doubt
have answered that this animal had been created with special instincts, adapting him for association with
man, and that all further enquiry on the subject was superfluous.
[12] `Anatomy of Expression,' 3rd edit. pp. 98, 121, 131.
[13] Professor Owen expressly states (Proc. Zoolog. Soc. 1830, p. 28) that this is the case with respect to the
Orang, and specifies all the more important muscles which are well known to serve with man for the
expression of his feelings. See, also, a description of several of the facial muscles in the Chimpanzee, by Prof.
Macalister, in `Annals and Magazine of Natural History,' vol. vii. May, 1871, p. 342.
[14] `Anatomy of Expression,' pp. 121, 138.
Although Gratiolet emphatically denies[15] that any muscle has been developed solely for the sake of
expression, he seems never to have reflected on the principle of evolution. He apparently looks at each
species as a separate creation. So it is with the other writers on Expression. For instance, Dr. Duchenne, after
speaking of the movements of the limbs, refers to those which give expression to the face, and remarks:[16]
"Le createur n'a donc pas eu a se preoccuper ici des besoins de la mecanique; il a pu, selon sa sagesse,
ouque l'on me pardonne cette maniere de parlerpar une divine fantaisie, mettre en action tel ou tel
muscle, un seul ou plusieurs muscles a la fois, lorsqu'il a voulu que les signes caracteristiques des passions,
meme les plus fugaces, lussent ecrits passagerement sur la face de l'homme. Ce langage de la physionomie
une fois cree, il lui a suffi, pour le rendre universel et immuable, de donner a tout etre humain la faculte
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 5
Page No 8
instinctive d'exprimer toujours ses sendments par la contraction des memes muscles."
Many writers consider the whole subject of Expression as inexplicable. Thus the illustrious physiologist
Muller, says,[17] "The completely different expression of the features in different passions shows that,
according to the kind of feeling excited, entirely different groups of the fibres of the facial nerve are acted on.
Of the cause of this we are quite ignorant."
[15] `De la Physionomie,' pp. 12, 73.
[16] `Mecanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,' 8vo edit. p. 31.
[17] `Elements of Physiology,' English translation, vol. ii. p. 934.
No doubt as long as man and all other animals are viewed as independent creations, an effectual stop is put to
our natural desire to investigate as far as possible the causes of Expression. By this doctrine, anything and
everything can be equally well explained; and it has proved as pernicious with respect to Expression as to
every other branch of natural history. With mankind some expressions, such as the bristling of the hair under
the influence of extreme terror, or the uncovering of the teeth under that of furious rage, can hardly be
understood, except on the belief that man once existed in a much lower and animallike condition. The
community of certain expressions in distinct though allied species, as in the movements of the same facial
muscles during laughter by man and by various monkeys, is rendered somewhat more intelligible, if we
believe in their descent from a common progenitor. He who admits on general grounds that the structure and
habits of all animals have been gradually evolved, will look at the whole subject of Expression in a new and
interesting light.
The study of Expression is difficult, owing to the movements being often extremely slight, and of a fleeting
nature. A difference may be clearly perceived, and yet it may be impossible, at least I have found it so, to
state in what the difference consists. When we witness any deep emotion, our sympathy is so strongly
excited, that close observation is forgotten or rendered almost impossible; of which fact I have had many
curious proofs. Our imagination is another and still more serious source of error; for if from the nature of the
circumstances we expect to see any expression, we readily imagine its presence. Notwithstanding Dr.
Duchenne's great experience, he for a long time fancied, as he states, that several muscles contracted under
certain emotions, whereas he ultimately convinced himself that the movement was confined to a single
muscle.
In order to acquire as good a foundation as possible, and to ascertain, independently of common opinion, how
far particular movements of the features and gestures are really expressive of certain states of the mind, I
have found the following means the most serviceable. In the first place, to observe infants; for they exhibit
many emotions, as Sir C. Bell remarks, "with extraordinary force;" whereas, in after life, some of our
expressions "cease to have the pure and simple source from which they spring in infancy."[18]
In the second place, it occurred to me that the insane ought to be studied, as they are liable to the strongest
passions, and give uncontrolled vent to them. I had, myself, no opportunity of doing this, so I applied to Dr.
Maudsley and received from him an introduction to Dr. J. Crichton Browne, who has charge of an immense
asylum near Wakefield, and who, as I found, had already attended to the subject. This excellent observer has
with unwearied kindness sent me copious notes and descriptions, with valuable suggestions on many points;
and I can hardly overestimate the value of his assistance. I owe also, to the kindness of Mr. Patrick Nicol, of
the Sussex Lunatic Asylum, interesting statements on two or three points.
Thirdly Dr. Duchenne galvanized, as we have already seen, certain muscles in the face of an old man, whose
skin was little sensitive, and thus produced various expressions which were photographed on a large scale. It
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 6
Page No 9
fortunately occurred to me to show several of the best plates, without a word of explanation, to above twenty
educated persons of various ages and both sexes, asking them, in each case, by what emotion or feeling the
old man was supposed to be agitated; and I recorded their answers in the words which they used. Several of
the expressions were instantly recognised by almost everyone, though described in not exactly the same
terms; and these may, I think, be relied on as truthful, and will hereafter be specified. On the other hand, the
most widely different judgments were pronounced in regard to some of them. This exhibition was of use in
another way, by convincing me how easily we may be misguided by our imagination; for when I first looked
through Dr. Duchenne's photographs, reading at the same time the text, and thus learning what was intended,
I was struck with admiration at the truthfulness of all, with only a few exceptions. Nevertheless, if I had
examined them without any explanation, no doubt I should have been as much perplexed, in some cases, as
other persons have been.
[18] "Anatomy of Expression,' 3rd edit. p. 198.
Fourthly, I had hoped to derive much aid from the great masters in painting and sculpture, who are such close
observers. Accordingly, I have looked at photographs and engravings of many wellknown works; but, with
a few exceptions, have not thus profited. The reason no doubt is, that in works of art, beauty is the chief
object; and strongly contracted facial muscles destroy beauty.[19] The story of the composition is generally
told with wonderful force and truth by skilfully given accessories.
[19] See remarks to this effect in Lessing's `Lacooon,' translated by W. Ross, 1836, p. 19.
Fifthly, it seemed to me highly important to ascertain whether the same expressions and gestures prevail, as
has often been asserted without much evidence, with all the races of mankind, especially with those who have
associated but little with Europeans. Whenever the same movements of the features or body express the same
emotions in several distinct races of man, we may infer with much probability, that such expressions are true
ones,that is, are innate or instinctive. Conventional expressions or gestures, acquired by the individual
during early life, would probably have differed in the different races, in the same manner as do their
languages. Accordingly I circulated, early in the year 1867, the following printed queries with a request,
which has been fully responded to, that actual observations, and not memory, might be trusted. These queries
were written after a considerable interval of time, during which my attention had been otherwise directed, and
I can now see that they might have been greatly improved. To some of the later copies, I appended, in
manuscript, a few additional remarks:
(1.) Is astonishment expressed by the eyes and mouth being opened wide, and by the eyebrows being raised?
(2.) Does shame excite a blush when the colour of the skin allows it to be visible? and especially how low
down the body does the blush extend?
(3.) When a man is indignant or defiant does he frown, hold his body and head erect, square his shoulders and
clench his fists?
(4) When considering deeply on any subject, or trying to understand any puzzle, does he frown, or wrinkle
the skin beneath the lower eyelids?
(5.) When in low spirits, are the corners of the mouth depressed, and the inner corner of the eyebrows raised
by that muscle which the French call the "Grief muscle"? The eyebrow in this state becomes slightly oblique,
with a little swelling at the Inner end; and the forehead is transversely wrinkled in the middle part, but not
across the whole breadth, as when the eyebrows are raised in surprise. (6.) When in good spirits do the eyes
sparkle, with the skin a little wrinkled round and under them, and with the mouth a little drawn back at the
corners?
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 7
Page No 10
(7.) When a man sneers or snarls at another, is the corner of the upper lip over the canine or eye tooth raised
on the side facing the man whom he addresses?
(8) Can a dogged or obstinate expression be recognized, which is chiefly shown by the mouth being firmly
closed, a lowering brow and a slight frown?
(9.) Is contempt expressed by a slight protrusion of the lips and by turning up the nose, and with a slight
expiration?
(10) Is disgust shown by the lower lip being turned down, the upper lip slightly raised, with a sudden
expiration, something like incipient vomiting, or like something spit out of the mouth?
(11.) Is extreme fear expressed in the same general manner as with Europeans?
(12.) Is laughter ever carried to such an extreme as to bring tears into the eyes?
(13.) When a man wishes to show that he cannot prevent something being done, or cannot himself do
something, does he shrug his shoulders, turn inwards his elbows, extend outwards his hands and open the
palms; with the eyebrows raised?
(14) Do the children when sulky, pout or greatly protrude the lips?
(15.) Can guilty, or sly, or jealous expressions be recognized? though I know not how these can be defined.
(16.) Is the head nodded vertically in affirmation, and shaken laterally in negation?
Observations on natives who have had little communication with Europeans would be of course the most
valuable, though those made on any natives would be of much interest to me. General remarks on expression
are of comparatively little value; and memory is so deceptive that I earnestly beg it may not be trusted. A
definite description of the countenance under any emotion or frame of mind, with a statement of the
circumstances under which it occurred, would possess much value.
To these queries I have received thirtysix answers from different observers, several of them missionaries or
protectors of the aborigines, to all of whom I am deeply indebted for the great trouble which they have taken,
and for the valuable aid thus received. I will specify their names, towards the close of this chapter, so as not
to interrupt my present remarks. The answers relate to several of the most distinct and savage races of man. In
many instances, the circumstances have been recorded under which each expression was observed, and the
expression itself described. In such cases, much confidence may be placed in the answers. When the answers
have been simply yes or no, I have always received them with caution. It follows, from the information thus
acquired, that the same state of mind is expressed throughout the world with remarkable uniformity; and this
fact is in itself interesting as evidence of the close similarity in bodily structure and mental disposition of all
the races, of mankind.
Sixthly, and lastly, I have attended. as closely as I could, to the expression of the several passions in some of
the commoner animals; and this I believe to be of paramount importance, not of course for deciding how far
in man certain expressions are characteristic of certain states of mind, but as affording the safest basis for
generalisation on the causes, or origin, of the various movements of Expression. In observing animals, we are
not so likely to be biassed by our imagination; and we may feel safe that their expressions are not
conventional.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 8
Page No 11
From the reasons above assigned, namely, the fleeting nature of some expressions (the changes in the features
being often extremely slight); our sympathy being easily aroused when we behold any strong emotion, and
our attention thus distracted; our imagination deceiving us, from knowing in a vague manner what to expect,
though certainly few of us know what the exact changes in the countenance are; and lastly, even our long
familiarity with the subject,from all these causes combined, the observation of Expression is by no means
easy, as many persons, whom I have asked to observe certain points, have soon discovered. Hence it is
difficult to determine, with certainty, what are the movements of the features and of the body, which
commonly characterize certain states of the mind. Nevertheless, some of the doubts and difficulties have, as I
hope, been cleared away by the observation of infants, of the insane,of the different races of man,of
works of art, and lastly, of the facial muscles under the action of galvanism, as effected by Dr. Duchenne.
But there remains the much greater difficulty of understanding the cause or origin of the several expressions,
and of judging whether any theoretical explanation is trustworthy. Besides, judging as well as we can by our
reason, without the aid of any rules, which of two or more explanations is the most satisfactory, or are quite
unsatisfactory, I see only one way of testing our conclusions. This is to observe whether the same principle
by which one expression can, as it appears, be explained, is applicable in other allied cases; and especially,
whether the same general principles can be applied with satisfactory results, both to man and the lower
animals. This latter method, I am inclined to think, is the most serviceable of all. The difficulty of judging of
the truth of any theoretical explanation, and of testing it by some distinct line of investigation, is the great
drawback to that interest which the study seems well fitted to excite.
Finally, with respect to my own observations, I may state that they were commenced in the year 1838; and
from that time to the present day, I have occasionally attended to the subject. At the above date, I was already
inclined to believe in the principle of evolution, or of the derivation of species from other and lower forms.
Consequently, when I read Sir C. Bell's great work, his view, that man had been created with certain muscles
specially adapted for the expression of his feelings, struck me as unsatisfactory. It seemed probable that the
habit of expressing our feelings by certain movements, though now rendered innate, had been in some
manner gradually acquired. But to discover how such habits had been acquired was perplexing in no small
degree. The whole subject had to be viewed under a new aspect, and each expression demanded a rational
explanation. This belief led me to attempt the present work, however imperfectly it may have been
executed.
I will now give the names of the gentlemen to whom, as I have said, I am deeply indebted for information in
regard to the expressions exhibited by various races of man, and I will specify some of the circumstances
under which the observations were in each case made. Owing to the great kindness and powerful influence of
Mr. Wilson, of Hayes Place, Kent, I have received from Australia no less than thirteen sets of answers to my
queries. This has been particularly fortunate, as the Australian aborigines rank amongst the most distinct of
all the races of man. It will be seen that the observations have been chiefly made in the south, in the outlying
parts of the colony of Victoria; but some excellent answers have been received from the north.
Mr. Dyson Lacy has given me in detail some valuable observations, made several hundred miles in the
interior of Queensland. To Mr. R. Brough Smyth, of Melbourne, I am much indebted for observations made
by himself, and for sending me several of the following letters, namely:From the Rev. Mr. Hagenauer, of
Lake Wellington, a missionary in Gippsland, Victoria, who has had much experience with the natives. From
Mr. Samuel Wilson, a landowner, residing at Langerenong, Wimmera, Victoria. From the Rev. George
Taplin, superintendent of the native Industrial Settlement at Port Macleay. From Mr. Archibald G. Lang, of
Coranderik, Victoria, a teacher at a school where aborigines, old and young, are collected from all parts of the
colony. From Mr. H. B. Lane, of Belfast, Victoria, a police magistrate and warden, whose observations, as I
am assured, are highly trustworthy. From Mr. Templeton Bunnett, of Echuca, whose station is on the borders
of the colony of Victoria, and who has thus been able to observe many aborigines who have had little
intercourse with white men. He compared his observations with those made by two other gentlemen long
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 9
Page No 12
resident in the neighbourhood. Also from Mr. J. Bulmer, a missionary in a remote part of Gippsland,
Victoria.
I am also indebted to the distinguished botanist, Dr. Ferdinand Muller, of Victoria, for some observations
made by himself, and for sending me others made by Mrs. Green, as well as for some of the foregoing letters.
In regard to the Maoris of New Zealand, the Rev. J. W. Stack has answered only a few of my queries; but the
answers have been remarkably full, clear, and distinct, with the circumstances recorded under which the
observations were made.
The Rajah Brooke has given me some information with respect to the Dyaks of Borneo.
Respecting the Malays, I have been highly successful; for Mr. F. Geach (to whom I was introduced by Mr.
Wallace), during his residence as a mining engineer in the interior of Malacca, observed many natives, who
had never before associated with white men. He wrote me two long letters with admirable and detailed
observations on their expression. He likewise observed the Chinese immigrants in the Malay archipelago.
The wellknown naturalist, H. M. Consul, Mr. Swinhoe, also observed for me the Chinese in their native
country; and he made inquiries from others whom he could trust.
In India Mr. H. Erskine, whilst residing in his official capacity in the Admednugur District in the Bombay
Presidency, attended to the expression of the inhabitants, but found much difficulty in arriving at any safe
conclusions, owing to their habitual concealment of all emotions in the presence of Europeans. He also
obtained information for me from Mr. West, the Judge in Canara, and he consulted some intelligent native
gentlemen on certain points. In Calcutta Mr. J. Scott, curator of the Botanic Gardens, carefully observed the
various tribes of men therein employed during a considerable period, and no one has sent me such full and
valuable details. The habit of accurate observation, gained by his botanical studies, has been brought to bear
on our present subject. For Ceylon I am much indebted to the Rev. S. O. Glenie for answers to some of my
queries.
Turning to Africa, I have been unfortunate with respect to the negroes, though Mr. Winwood Reade aided me
as far as lay in his power. It would have been comparatively easy to have obtained information in regard to
the negro slaves in America; but as they have long associated with white men, such observations would have
possessed little value. In the southern parts of the continent Mrs. Barber observed the Kafirs and Fingoes, and
sent me many distinct answers. Mr. J. P. Mansel Weale also made some observations on the natives, and
procured for me a curious document, namely, the opinion, written in English, of Christian Gaika, brother of
the Chief Sandilli, on the expressions of his fellowcountrymen. In the northern regions of Africa Captain
Speedy, who long resided with the Abyssinians, answered my queries partly from memory and partly from
observations made on the son of King Theodore, who was then under his charge. Professor and Mrs. Asa
Gray attended to some points in the expressions of the natives, as observed by them whilst ascending the
Nile.
On the great American continent Mr. Bridges, a catechist residing with the Fuegians, answered some few
questions about their expression, addressed to him many years ago. In the northern half of the continent Dr.
Rothrock attended to the expressions of the wild Atnah and Espyox tribes on the Nasse River, in
NorthWestern America. Mr. Washington Matthews AssistantSurgeon in the United States Army, also
observed with special care (after having seen my queries, as printed in the `Smithsonian Report') some of the
wildest tribes in the Western parts of the United States, namely, the Tetons, Grosventres, Mandans, and
Assinaboines; and his answers have proved of the highest value.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals 10
Page No 13
Lastly, besides these special sources of information, I have collected some few facts incidentally given in
books of travels.
As I shall often have to refer, more especially in the latter part of this volume, to the muscles of the human
face, I have had a diagram (fig. 1) copied and reduced from Sir C. Bell's work, and two others, with more
accurate details (figs. 2 and 3), from Herde's wellknown `Handbuch der Systematischen Anatomie des
Menschen.' The same letters refer to the same muscles in all three figures, but the names are given of only the
more important ones to which I shall have to allude. The facial muscles blend much together, and, as I am
informed, hardly appear on a dissected face so distinct as they are here represented. Some writers consider
that these muscles consist of nineteen pairs, with one unpaired;[20] but others make the number much larger,
amounting even to fiftyfive, according to Moreau. They are, as is admitted by everyone who has written on
the subject, very variable in structure; and Moreau remarks that they are hardly alike in halfadozen
subjects.[21] They are also variable in function. Thus the power of uncovering the canine tooth on one side
differs much in different persons. The power of raising the wings of the nostrils is also, according to Dr.
Piderit,[22] variable in a remarkable degree; and other such cases could be given.
[20] Mr. Partridge in Todd's `Cyclopaedia of Anatomy and Physiology,' vol. ii. p. 227.
[21] `La Physionomie,' par G. Lavater, tom. iv. 1820, p. 274. On the number of the facial muscles, see vol. iv.
pp. 209211.
[22] " `Mimik und Physiognomik,' 1867, s. 91.
Finally, I must have the pleasure of expressing my obligations to Mr. Rejlander for the trouble which he has
taken in photographing for me various expressions and gestures. I am also indebted to Herr Kindermann, of
Hamburg, for the loan of some excellent negatives of crying infants; and to Dr. Wallich for a charming one of
a smiling girl. I have already expressed my obligations to Dr. Duchenne for generously permitting me to have
some of his large photographs copied and reduced. All these photographs have been printed by the Heliotype
process, and the accuracy of the copy is thus guaranteed. These plates are referred to by Roman numerals.
I am also greatly indebted to Mr. T. W. Wood for the extreme pains which he has taken in drawing from life
the expressions of various animals. A distinguished artist, Mr. Riviere, has had the kindness to give me two
drawings of dogsone in a hostile and the other in a humble and caressing frame of mind. Mr. A. May has
also given me two similar sketches of dogs. Mr. Cooper has taken much care in cutting the blocks. Some of
the photographs and drawings, namely, those by Mr. May, and those by Mr. Wolf of the Cynopithecus, were
first reproduced by Mr. Cooper on wood by means of photography, and then engraved: by this means almost
complete fidelity is ensured.
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION.
The three chief principles statedThe first principleServiceable actions become habitual in association
with certain states of the mind, and are performed whether or not of service in each particular case The
force of habitInheritanceAssociated habitual movements in man Reflex actionsPassage of habits
into reflex actionsAssociated habitual movements in the lower animalsConcluding remarks.
I WILL begin by giving the three Principles, which appear to me to account for most of the expressions and
gestures involuntarily used by man and the lower animals, under the influence of various emotions and
sensations.[1] I arrived, however, at these three Principles only at the close of my observations. They will be
discussed in the present and two following chapters in a general manner. Facts observed both with man and
the lower animals will here be made use of; but the latter facts are preferable, as less likely to deceive us. In
the fourth and fifth chapters, I will describe the special expressions of some of the lower animals; and in the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 11
Page No 14
succeeding chapters those of man. Everyone will thus be able to judge for himself, how far my three
principles throw light on the theory of the subject. It appears to me that so many expressions are thus
explained in a fairly satisfactory manner, that probably all will hereafter be found to come under the same or
closely analogous heads. I need hardly premise that movements or changes in any part of the body, as the
wagging of a dog's tail, the drawing back of a horse's ears, the shrugging of a man's shoulders, or the
dilatation of the capillary vessels of the skin,may all equally well serve for expression. The three Principles
are as follows.
[1] Mr. Herbert Spencer (`Essays,' Second Series, 1863, p. 138) has drawn a clear distinction between
emotions and sensations, the latter being "generated in our corporeal framework." He classes as Feelings both
emotions andsensations.
I. _The principle of serviceable associated Habits_.Certain complex actions are of direct or indirect service
under certain states of the mind, in order to relieve or gratify certain sensations, desires, and whenever the
same state of mind is induced, however feebly, there is a tendency through the force of habit and association
for the same movements to be performed, though they may not then be of the least use. Some actions
ordinarily associated through habit with certain states of the mind may be partially repressed through the will,
and in such cases the muscles which are least under the separate control of the will are the most liable still to
act, causing movements which we recognize as expressive. In certain other cases the checking of one habitual
movement requires other slight movements; and these are likewise expressive.
II. _The principle of Antithesis_.Certain states of the mind lead to certain habitual actions, which are of
service, as under our first principle. Now when a directly opposite state of mind is induced, there is a strong
and involuntary tendency to the performance of movements of a directly opposite nature, though these are of
no use; and such movements are in some cases highly expressive.
III. _The principle of actions due to the constitution of the Nervous System, independently from the first of
the Will, and independently to a certain extent of Habit_. When the sensorium is strongly excited,
nerveforce is generated in excess, and is transmitted in certain definite directions, depending on the
connection of the nervecells, and partly on habit: or the supply of nerveforce may, as it appears, be
interrupted. Effects are thus produced which we recognize as expressive. This third principle may, for the
sake of brevity, be called that of the direct action of the nervous system.
With respect to our _first Principle_, it is notorious how powerful is the force of habit. The most complex and
difficult movements can in time be performed without the least effort or consciousness. It is not positively
known how it comes that habit is so efficient in facilitating complex movements; but physiologists admit[2]
"that the conducting power of the nervous fibres increases with the frequency of their excitement." This
applies to the nerves of motion and sensation, as well as to those connected with the act of thinking. That
some physical change is produced in the nervecells or nerves which are habitually used can hardly be
doubted, for otherwise it is impossible to understand how the tendency to certain acquired movements is
inherited. That they are inherited we see with horses in certain transmitted paces, such as cantering and
ambling, which are not natural to them, in the pointing of young pointers and the setting of young
setters in the peculiar manner of flight of certain breeds of the pigeon, We have analogous cases with
mankind in the inheritance of tricks or unusual gestures, to which we shall presently recur. To those who
admit the gradual evolution of species, a most striking instance of the perfection with which the most difficult
consensual movements can be transmitted, is afforded by the hummingbird Sphinxmoth (_Macroglossa_);
for this moth, shortly after its emergence from the cocoon, as shown by the bloom on its unruffled scales,
may be seen poised stationary in the air, with its long hairlike proboscis uncurled and inserted into the
minute orifices of flowers; and no one, I believe, has ever seen this moth learning to perform its difficult task,
which requires such unerring aim.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 12
Page No 15
[2] Muller, `Elements of Physiology,' Eng. translat. vol. ii. p. 939. See also Mr. H. Spencer's interesting
speculations on the same subject, and on the genesis of nerves, in his `Principles of Biology,' vol. ii. p. 346;
and in his `Principles of Psychology,' 2nd edit. pp. 511557.
When there exists an inherited or instinctive tendency to the performance of an action, or an inherited taste
for certain kinds of food, some degree of habit in the individual is often or generally requisite. We find this in
the paces of the horse, and to a certain extent in the pointing of dogs; although some young dogs point
excellently the first time they are taken out, yet they often associate the proper inherited attitude with a wrong
odour, and even with eyesight. I have heard it asserted that if a calf be allowed to suck its mother only once, it
is much more difficult afterwards to rear it by hand.[3] Caterpillars which have been fed on the leaves of one
kind of tree, have been known to perish from hunger rather than to eat the leaves of another tree, although
this afforded them their proper food, under a state of nature;[4] and so it is in many other cases.
[3] A remark to much the same effect was made long ago by Hippocrates and by the illustrious Harvey; for
both assert that a young animal forgets in the course of a few days the art of sucking, and cannot without
some difficulty again acquire it. I give these assertions on the authority of Dr. Darwin, `Zoonomia,' 1794, vol.
i. p. 140.
The power of Association is admitted by everyone. Mr. Bain remarks, that "actions, sensations and states of
feeling, occurring together or in close succession, tend to grow together, or cohere, in such a way that when
any one of them is afterwards presented to the mind, the others are apt to be brought up in idea."[5] It is so
important for our purpose fully to recognize that actions readily become associated with other actions and
with various states of the mind, that I will give a good many instances, in the first place relating to man, and
afterwards to the lower animals. Some of the instances are of a very trifling nature, but they are as good for
our purpose as more important habits. It is known to everyone how difficult, or even impossible it is, without
repeated trials, to move the limbs in certain opposed directions which have never been practised. Analogous
cases occur with sensations, as in the common experiment of rolling a marble beneath the tips of two crossed
fingers, when it feels exactly like two marbles. Everyone protects himself when falling to the ground by
extending his arms, and as Professor Alison has remarked, few can resist acting thus, when voluntarily falling
on a soft bed. A man when going out of doors puts on his gloves quite unconsciously; and this may seem an
extremely simple operation, but he who has taught a child to put on gloves, knows that this is by no means
the case.
[4] See for my authorities, and for various analogous facts, `The Variation of Animals and Plants under
Domestication,' 1868, vol. ii. p. 304.
[5] `The Senses and the Intellect,' 2nd edit. 1864, p. 332. Prof. Huxley remarks (`Elementary Lessons in
Physiology,' 5th edit. 1872, p. 306), "It may be laid down as a rule, that, if any two mental states be called up
together, or in succession, with due frequency and vividness, the subsequent production of the one of them
will suffice to call up the other, and that whether we desire it or not."
When our minds are much affected, so are the movements of our bodies; but here another principle besides
habit, namely the undirected overflow of nerveforce, partially comes into play. Norfolk, in speaking of
Cardinal Wolsey, says
"Some strange commotion
Is in his brain; he bites his lip and starts;
Stops on a sudden, looks upon the ground,
Then, lays his finger on his temple: straight,
Springs out into fast gait; then, stops again,
Strikes his breast hard; and anon, he casts
His eye against the moon: in most strange postures
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 13
Page No 16
We have seen him set himself."_Hen. VIII_., act 3, sc. 2.
A vulgar man often scratches his head when perplexed in mind; and I believe that he acts thus from habit, as
if he experienced a slightly uncomfortable bodily sensation, namely, the itching of his head, to which he is
particularly liable, and which he thus relieves. Another man rubs his eyes when perplexed, or gives a little
cough when embarrassed, acting in either case as if he felt a slightly uncomfortable sensation in his eyes or
windpipe.[6]
From the continued use of the eyes, these organs are especially liable to be acted on through association
under various states of the mind, although there is manifestly nothing to be seen. A man, as Gratiolet
remarks, who vehemently rejects a proposition, will almost certainly shut his eyes or turn away his face; but
if he accepts the proposition, he will nod his head in affirmation and open his eyes widely. The man acts in
this latter case as if he clearly saw the thing, and in the former case as if he did not or would not see it. I have
noticed that persons in describing a horrid sight often shut their eyes momentarily and firmly, or shake their
heads, as if not to see or to drive away something disagreeable; and I have caught myself, when thinking in
the dark of a horrid spectacle, closing my eyes firmly. In looking suddenly at any object, or in looking all
around, everyone raises his eyebrows, so that the eyes may be quickly and widely opened; and Duchenne
remarks that[7] a person in trying to remember something often raises his eyebrows, as if to see it. A Hindoo
gentleman made exactly the same remark to Mr. Erskine in regard to his countrymen. I noticed a young lady
earnestly trying to recollect a painter's name, and she first looked to one corner of the ceiling and then to the
opposite corner, arching the one eyebrow on that side; although, of course, there was nothing to be seen there.
[6] Gratiolet (`De la Physionomie,' p. 324), in his discussion on this subject, gives many analogous instances.
See p. 42, on the opening and shutting of the eyes. Engel is quoted (p. 323) on the changed paces of a man, as
his thoughts change.
In most of the foregoing cases, we can understand how the associated movements were acquired through
habit; but with some individuals, certain strange gestures or tricks have arisen in association with certain
states of the mind, owing to wholly inexplicable causes, and are undoubtedly inherited. I have elsewhere
given one instance from my own observation of an extraordinary and complex gesture, associated with
pleasurable feelings, which was transmitted from a father to his daughter, as well as some other analogous
facts.[8]
[7] `Mecanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,' 1862, p. 17.
[8] `The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,' vol. ii. p. 6. The inheritance of habitual
gestures is so important for us, that I gladly avail myself of Mr. F. Galton's permission to give in his own
words the following remarkable case:"The following account of a habit occurring in individuals of three
consecutive generations {footnote continues:} is of peculiar interest, because it occurs only during sound
sleep, and therefore cannot be due to imitation, but must be altogether natural. The particulars are perfectly
trustworthy, for I have enquired fully into them, and speak from abundant and independent evidence. A
gentleman of considerable position was found by his wife to have the curious trick, when he lay fast asleep
on his back in bed, of raising his right arm slowly in front of his face, up to his forehead, and then dropping it
with a jerk, so that the wrist fell heavily on the bridge of his nose. The trick did not occur every night, but
occasionally, and was independent of any ascertained cause. Sometimes it was repeated incessantly for an
hour or more. The gentleman's nose was prominent, and its bridge often became sore from the blows which it
received. At one time an awkward sore was produced, that was long in healing, on account of the recurrence,
night after night, of the blows which first caused it. His wife had to remove the button from the wrist of his
nightgown as it made severe scratches, and some means were attempted of tying his arm.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 14
Page No 17
"Many years after his death, his son married a lady who had never heard of the family incident. She,
however, observed precisely the same peculiarity in her husband; but his nose, from not being particularly
prominent, has never as yet suffered from the blows. The trick does not occur when he is halfasleep, as, for
example, when dozing in his armchair, but the moment he is fast asleep it is apt to begin. It is, as with his
father, intermittent; sometimes ceasing for many nights, and sometimes almost incessant during a part of
every night. It is performed, as it was by his father, with his right hand.
"One of his children, a girl, has inherited the same trick. She performs it, likewise, with the right hand, but in
a slightly modified form; for, after raising the arm, she does not allow the wrist to drop upon the bridge of the
nose, but the palm of the halfclosed hand falls over and down the nose, striking it rather rapidly. It is also
very intermittent with this child, not occurring for periods of some months, but sometimes occurring almost
incessantly." {end of long footnote}
Another curious instance of an odd inherited movement, associated with the wish to obtain an object, will be
given in the course of this volume.
There are other actions which are commonly performed under certain circumstances, independently of habit,
and which seem to be due to imitation or some sort of sympathy. Thus persons cutting anything with a pair of
scissors may be seen to move their jaws simultaneously with the blades of the scissors. Children learning to
write often twist about their tongues as their fingers move, in a ridiculous fashion. When a public singer
suddenly becomes a little hoarse, many of those present may be heard, as I have been assured by a gentleman
on whom I can rely, to clear their throats; but here habit probably comes into play, as we clear our own
throats under similar circumstances. I have also been told that at leaping matches, as the performer makes his
spring, many of the spectators, generally men and boys, move their feet; but here again habit probably comes
into play, for it is very doubtful whether women would thus act.
_Reflex actions_Reflex actions, in the strict sense of the term, are due to the excitement of a peripheral
nerve, which transmits its influence to certain nervecells, and these in their turn excite certain muscles or
glands into action; and all this may take place without any sensation or consciousness on our part, though
often thus accompanied. As many reflex actions are highly expressive, the subject must here be noticed at
some little length. We shall also see that some of them graduate into, and can hardly be distinguished from
actions which have arisen through habit? Coughing and sneezing are familiar instances of reflex actions.
With infants the first act of respiration is often a sneeze, although this requires the coordinated movement of
numerous muscles. Respiration is partly voluntary, but mainly reflex, and is performed in the most natural
and best manner without the interference of the will. A vast number of complex movements are reflex. As
good an instance as can be given is the oftenquoted one of a decapitated frog, which cannot of course feel,
and cannot consciously perform, any movement. Yet if a drop of acid be placed on the lower surface of the
thigh of a frog in this state, it will rub off the drop with the upper surface of the foot of the same leg. If this
foot be cut off, it cannot thus act. "After some fruitless efforts, therefore, it gives up trying in that way, seems
restless, as though, says Pfluger, it was seeking some other way, and at last it makes use of the foot of the
other leg and succeeds in rubbing off the acid. Notably we have here not merely contractions of muscles, but
combined and harmonized contractions in due sequence for a special purpose. These are actions that have all
the appearance of being guided by intelligence and instigated by will in an animal, the recognized organ of
whose intelligence and will has been removed."[10]
[9] Prof. Huxley remarks (`Elementary Physiology,' 5th edit. p. 305) that reflex actions proper to the spinal
cord are NATURAL; but, by the help of the brain, that is through habit, an infinity of ARTIFICIAL reflex
actions may be acquired. Virchow admits (`Sammlung wissenschaft. Vortrage,' "Ueber das Ruckeninark,"
1871, ss. 24, 31) that some reflex actions can hardly be distinguished from instincts; and, of the latter, it may
be added, some cannot be distinguished from inherited habits.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 15
Page No 18
We see the difference between reflex and voluntary movements in very young children not being able to
perform, as I am informed by Sir Henry Holland, certain acts somewhat analogous to those of sneezing and
coughing, namely, in their not being able to blow their noses (i. e. to compress the nose and blow violently
through the passage), and in their not being able to clear their throats of phlegm. They have to learn to
perform these acts, yet they are performed by us, when a little older, almost as easily as reflex actions.
Sneezing and coughing, however, can be controlled by the will only partially or not at all; whilst the clearing
the throat and blowing the nose are completely under our command.
[10] "Dr. Maudsley, `Body and Mind,' 1870, p. 8.
When we are conscious of the presence of an irritating particle in our nostrils or windpipethat is, when the
same sensory nervecells are excited, as in the case of sneezing and coughing we can voluntarily expel the
particle by forcibly driving air through these passages; but we cannot do this with nearly the same force,
rapidity, and precision, as by a reflex action. In this latter case the sensory nervecells apparently excite the
motor nervecells without any waste of power by first communicating with the cerebral hemispheresthe
seat of our consciousness and volition. In all cases there seems to exist a profound antagonism between the
same movements, as directed by the will and by a reflex stimulant, in the force with which they are
performed and in the facility with which they are excited. As Claude Bernard asserts, "L'influence du cerveau
tend donc a entraver les mouvements reflexes, a limiter leur force et leur etendue."[11]
The conscious wish to perform a reflex action sometimes stops or interrupts its performance, though the
proper sensory nerves may be stimulated. For instance, many years ago I laid a small wager with a dozen
young men that they would not sneeze if they took snuff, although they all declared that they invariably did
so; accordingly they all took a pinch, but from wishing much to succeed, not one sneezed, though their eyes
watered, and all, without exception, had to pay me the wager. Sir H. Holland remarks[12] that attention paid
to the act of swallowing interferes with the proper movements; from which it probably follows, at least in
part, that some persons find it so difficult to swallow a pill.
[11] "See the very interesting discussion on the whole subject by Claude Bernard, `Tissus Vivants,' 1866, p.
353356.
[12] `Chapters on Mental Physiology,' 1858, p. 85.
Another familiar instance of a reflex action is the involuntary closing of the eyelids when the surface of the
eye is touched. A similar winking movement is caused when a blow is directed towards the face; but this is an
habitual and not a strictly reflex action, as the stimulus is conveyed through the mind and not by the
excitement of a peripheral nerve. The whole body and head are generally at the same time drawn suddenly
backwards. These latter movements, however, can be prevented, if the danger does not appear to the
imagination imminent; but our reason telling us that there is no danger does not suffice. I may mention a
trifling fact, illustrating this point, and which at the time amused me. I put my face close to the thick
glassplate in front of a puffadder in the Zoological Gardens, with the firm determination of not starting
back if the snake struck at me; but, as soon as the blow was struck, my resolution went for nothing, and I
jumped a yard or two backwards with astonishing rapidity. My will and reason were powerless against the
imagination of a danger which had never been experienced.
The violence of a start seems to depend partly on the vividness of the imagination, and partly on the
condition, either habitual or temporary, of the nervous system. He who will attend to the starting of his horse,
when tired and fresh, will perceive how perfect is the gradation from a mere glance at some unexpected
object, with a momentary doubt whether it is dangerous, to a jump so rapid and violent, that the animal
probably could not voluntarily whirl round in so rapid a manner. The nervous system of a fresh and
highlyfed horse sends its order to the motory system so quickly, that no time is allowed for him to consider
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 16
Page No 19
whether or not the danger is real. After one violent start, when he is excited and the blood flows freely
through his brain, he is very apt to start again; and so it is, as I have noticed, with young infants.
A start from a sudden noise, when the stimulus is conveyed through the auditory nerves, is always
accompanied in grownup persons by the winking of the eyelids.[13] I observed, however, that though my
infants started at sudden sounds, when under a fortnight old, they certainly did not always wink their eyes,
and I believe never did so. The start of an older infant apparently represents a vague catching hold of
something to prevent falling. I shook a pasteboard box close before the eyes of one of my infants, when 114
days old, and it did not in the least wink; but when I put a few comfits into the box, holding it in the same
position as before, and rattled them, the child blinked its eyes violently every time, and started a little. It was
obviously impossible that a carefullyguarded infant could have learnt by experience that a rattling sound
near its eyes indicated danger to them. But such experience will have been slowly gained at a later age during
a long series of generations; and from what we know of inheritance, there is nothing improbable in the
transmission of a habit to the offspring at an earlier age than that at which it was first acquired by the parents.
From the foregoing remarks it seems probable that some actions, which were at first performed consciously,
have become through habit and association converted into reflex actions, and are now so firmly fixed and
inherited, that they are performed, even when not of the least use,[14] as often as the same causes arise,
which originally excited them in us through the volition. In such cases the sensory nervecells excite the
motor cells, without first communicating with those cells on which our consciousness and volition depend. It
is probable that sneezing and coughing were originally acquired by the habit of expelling, as violently as
possible, any irritating particle from the sensitive airpassages. As far as time is concerned, there has been
more than enough for these habits to have become innate or converted into reflex actions; for they are
common to most or all of the higher quadrupeds, and must therefore have been first acquired at a very remote
period. Why the act of clearing the throat is not a reflex action, and has to be learnt by our children, I cannot
pretend to say; but we can see why blowing the nose on a handkerchief has to be learnt.
[13] Muller remarks (`Elements of Physiology,' Eng. tr. vol. ii. p. 1311) on starting being always
accompanied by the closure of the eyelids.
[14] Dr. Maudsley remarks (`Body and Mind,' p. 10) that "reflex movements which commonly effect a useful
end may, under the changed circumstances of disease, do great mischief, becoming even the occasion of
violent suffering and of a most painful death."
It is scarcely credible that the movements of a headless frog, when it wipes off a drop of acid or other object
from its thigh, and which movements are so well coordinated for a special purpose, were not at first
performed voluntarily, being afterwards rendered easy through longcontinued habit so as at last to be
performed unconsciously, or independently of the cerebral hemispheres.
So again it appears probable that starting was originally acquired by the habit of jumping away as quickly as
possible from danger, whenever any of our senses gave us warning. Starting, as we have seen, is
accompanied by the blinking of the eyelids so as to protect the eyes, the most tender and sensitive organs of
the body; and it is, I believe, always accompanied by a sudden and forcible inspiration, which is the natural
preparation for any violent effort. But when a man or horse starts, his heart beats wildly against his ribs, and
here it may be truly said we have an organ which has never been under the control of the will, partaking in
the general reflex movements of the body. To this point, however, I shall return in a future chapter.
The contraction of the iris, when the retina is stimulated by a bright light, is another instance of a movement,
which it appears cannot possibly have been at first voluntarily performed and then fixed by habit; for the iris
is not known to be under the conscious control of the will in any animal. In such cases some explanation,
quite distinct from habit, will have to be discovered. The radiation of nerveforce from stronglyexcited
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 17
Page No 20
nervecells to other connected cells, as in the case of a bright light on the retina causing a sneeze, may
perhaps aid us in understanding how some reflex actions originated. A radiation of nerveforce of this kind,
if it caused a movement tending to lessen the primary irritation, as in the case of the contraction of the iris
preventing too much light from falling on the retina, might afterwards have been taken advantage of and
modified for this special purpose.
It further deserves notice that reflex actions are in all probability liable to slight variations, as are all corporeal
structures and instincts; and any variations which were beneficial and of sufficient importance, would tend to
be preserved and inherited. Thus reflex actions, when once gained for one purpose, might afterwards be
modified independently of the will or habit, so as to serve for some distinct purpose. Such cases would be
parallel with those which, as we have every reason to believe, have occurred with many instincts; for
although some instincts have been developed simply through longcontinued and inherited habit, other
highly complex ones have been developed through the preservation of variations of preexisting instincts
that is, through natural selection.
I have discussed at some little length, though as I am well aware, in a very imperfect manner, the acquirement
of reflex actions, because they are often brought into play in connection with movements expressive of our
emotions; and it was necessary to show that at least some of them might have been Erst acquired through the
will in order to satisfy a desire, or to relieve a disagreeable sensation.
_Associated habitual movements in the lower animals_. I have already given in the case of Man several
instances of movements associated with various states of the mind or body, which are now purposeless, but
which were originally of use, and are still of use under certain circumstances. As this subject is very
important for us, I will here give a considerable number of analogous facts, with reference to animals;
although many of them are of a very trifling nature. My object is to show that certain movements were
originally performed for a definite end, and that, under nearly the same circumstances, they are still
pertinaciously performed through habit when not of the least use. That the tendency in most of the following
cases is inherited, we may infer from such actions being performed in the same manner by all the individuals,
young and old, of he same species. We shall also see that they are excited by the most diversified, often
circuitous, and sometimes mistaken associations.
Dogs, when they wish to go to sleep on a carpet or other hard surface, generally turn round and round and
scratch the ground with their forepaws in a senseless manner, as if they intended to trample down the grass
and scoop out a hollow, as no doubt their wild parents did, when they lived on open grassy plains or in the
woods. Jackals, fennecs, and other allied animals in the Zoological Gardens, treat their straw in this manner;
but it is a rather odd circumstance that the keepers, after observing for some months, have never seen the
wolves thus behave. A semiidiotic dogand an animal in this condition would be particularly liable to
follow a senseless habitwas observed by a friend to turn completely round on a carpet thirteen times before
going to sleep.
Many carnivorous animals, as they crawl towards their prey and prepare to rush or spring on it, lower their
heads and crouch, partly, as it would appear, to hide themselves, and partly to get ready for their rush; and
this habit in an exaggerated form has become hereditary in our pointers and setters. Now I have noticed
scores of times that when two strange dogs meet on an open road, the one which first sees the other, though at
the distance of one or two hundred yards, after the first glance always lowers its bead, generally crouches a
little, or even lies down; that is, he takes the proper attitude for concealing himself and {illust. caption = for
making a rush or FIG. 4.Small dog watching a cat on a spring, although the road table. From a photograph
taken is quite open and The distance Mr. Rejlander.} great. Again, dogs of all kinds when intently watching
and slowly approaching their prey, frequently keep one of their forelegs doubled up for a long time, ready
for the next cautious step; and this is eminently characteristic of the pointer. But from habit they behave in
exactly the same manner whenever their attention is aroused (fig. 4). I have seen a dog at the foot of a high
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 18
Page No 21
wall, listening attentively to a sound on the opposite side, with one leg doubled up; and in this case there
could have been no intention of making a cautious approach.
Dogs after voiding their excrement often make with all four feet a few scratches backwards, even on a bare
stone pavement, as if for the purpose of covering up their excrement with earth, in nearly the same manner as
do cats. Wolves and jackals behave in the Zoological Gardens in exactly the same manner, yet, as I am
assured by the keepers, neither wolves, jackals, nor foxes, when they have the means of doing so, ever cover
up their excrement, any more than do dogs. All these animals, however, bury superfluous food. Hence, if we
rightly understand the meaning of the above catlike habit, of which there can be little doubt, we have a
purposeless remnant of an habitual movement, which was originally followed by some remote progenitor of
the doggenus for a definite purpose, and which has been retained for a prodigious length of time.
Dogs and jackals[15] take much pleasure in rolling and rubbing their necks and backs on carrion. The odour
seems delightful to them, though dogs at least do not eat carrion. Mr. Bartlett has observed wolves for me,
and has given them carrion, but has never seen them roll on it. I have heard it remarked, and I believe it to be
true, that the larger dogs, which are probably descended from wolves, do not so often roll in carrion as do
smaller dogs, which are probably descended from jackals. When a piece of brown biscuit is offered to a
terrier of mine and she is not hungry (and I have heard of similar instances), she first tosses it about and
worries it, as if it were a rat or other prey; she then repeatedly rolls on it precisely as if it were a piece of
carrion, and at last eats it. It would appear that an imaginary relish has to be given to the distasteful morsel;
and to effect this the dog acts in his habitual manner, as if the biscuit was a live animal or smelt like carrion,
though he knows better than we do that this is not the case. I have seen this same terrier act in the same
manner after killing a little bird or mouse.
[15] See Mr. F. H. Salvin's account of a tame jackal in `Land and Water,' October, 1869.
Dogs scratch themselves by a rapid movement of one of their hindfeet; and when their backs are rubbed
with a stick, so strong is the habit, that they cannot help rapidly scratching the air or the ground in a useless
and ludicrous manner. The terrier just alluded to, when thus scratched with a stick, will sometimes show her
delight by another habitual movement, namely, by licking the air as if it were my hand.
Horses scratch themselves by nibbling those parts of their bodies which they can reach with their teeth; but
more commonly one horse shows another where he wants to be scratched, and they then nibble each other. A
friend whose attention I had called to the subject, observed that when he rubbed his horse's neck, the animal
protruded his head, uncovered his teeth, and moved his jaws, exactly as if nibbling another horse's neck, for
he could never have nibbled his own neck. If a horse is much tickled, as when currycombed, his wish to bite
something becomes so intolerably strong, that he will clatter his teeth together, and though not vicious, bite
his groom. At the same time from habit he closely depresses his ears, so as to protect them from being bitten,
as if he were fighting with another horse.
A horse when eager to start on a journey makes the nearest approach which he can to the habitual movement
of progression by pawing the ground. Now when horses in their stalls are about to be fed and are eager for
their corn, they paw the pavement or the straw. Two of my horses thus behave when they see or hear the corn
given to their neighbours. But here we have what may almost be called a true expression, as pawing the
ground is universally recognized as a sign of eagerness.
Cats cover up their excrements of both kinds with earth; and my grandfather[17]{sic} saw a kitten scraping
ashes over a spoonful of pure water spilt on the hearth; so that here an habitual or instinctive action was
falsely excited, not by a previous act or by odour, but by eyesight. It is well known that cats dislike wetting
their feet, owing, it is probable, to their having aboriginally inhabited the dry country of Egypt; and when
they wet their feet they shake them violently. My daughter poured some water into a glass close to the head
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 19
Page No 22
of a kitten; and it immediately shook its feet in the usual manner; so that here we have an habitual movement
falsely excited by an associated sound instead of by the sense of touch.
Kittens, puppies, young pigs and probably many other young animals, alternately push with their forefeet
against the mammary glands of their mothers, to excite a freer secretion of milk, or to make it flow. Now it is
very common with young cats, and not at all rare with old cats of the common and Persian breeds (believed
by some naturalists to be specifically extinct), when comfortably lying on a warm shawl or other soft
substance, to pound it quietly and alternately with their forefeet; their toes being spread out and claws
slightly protruded, precisely as when sucking their mother. That it is the same movement is clearly shown by
their often at the same time taking a bit of the shawl into their mouths and sucking it; generally closing their
eyes and purring from delight. This curious movement is commonly excited only in association with the
sensation of a warm soft surface; but I have seen an old cat, when pleased by having its back scratched,
pounding the air with its feet in the same manner; so that this action has almost become the expression of a
pleasurable sensation.
[16]"Dr. Darwin, `Zoonomia,' 1794, vol. i. p. 160. I find that the fact of cats protruding their feet when
pleased is also noticed (p. 151) in this work.
Having referred to the act of sucking, I may add that this complex movement, as well as the alternate
protrusion of the forefeet, are reflex actions; for they are performed if a finger moistened with milk is placed
in the mouth of a puppy, the front part of whose brain has been removed.[17] It has recently been stated in
France, that the action of sucking is excited solely through the sense of smell, so that if the olfactory nerves
of a puppy are destroyed, it never sucks. In like manner the wonderful power which a chicken possesses only
a few hours after being hatched, of picking up small particles of food, seems to be started into action through
the sense of hearing; for with chickens hatched by artificial heat, a good observer found that "making a noise
with the fingernail against a board, in imitation of the henmother, first taught them to peck at their
meat."[18]
[17] Carpenter, `Principles of Comparative Physiology,' 1854, p. 690, and Muller's `Elements of Physiology,'
Eng. translat. vol. ii. p. 936.
[18] Mowbray on `Poultry,' 6th edit. 1830, p. 54.
I will give only one other instance of an habitual and purposeless movement. The Sheldrake (_Tadorna_)
feeds on the sands left uncovered by the tide, and when a wormcast is discovered, "it begins patting the
ground with its feet, dancing as it were, over the hole;" and this makes the worm come to the surface. Now
Mr. St. John says, that when his tame Sheldrakes "came to ask for food, they patted the ground in an
impatient and rapid manner."[19] This therefore may almost be considered as their expression of hunger. Mr.
Bartlett informs me that the Flamingo and the Kagu (_Rhinochetus jubatus_) when anxious to be fed, beat the
ground with their feet in the same odd manner. So again Kingfishers, when they catch a fish, always beat it
until it is killed; and in the Zoological Gardens they always beat the raw meat, with which they are sometimes
fed, before devouring it.
We have now, I think, sufficiently shown the truth of our first Principle, namely, that when any sensation,
desire, dislike, has led during a long series of generations to some voluntary movement, then a tendency to
the performance of a similar movement will almost certainly be excited, whenever the same, or any
analogous or associated sensation although very weak, is experienced; notwithstanding that the movement in
this case may not be of the least use. Such habitual movements are often, or generally inherited; and they then
differ but little from reflex actions. When we treat of the special expressions of man, the latter part of our first
Principle, as given at the commencement of this chapter, will be seen to hold good; namely, that when
movements, associated through habit with certain states of the mind, are partially repressed by the will, the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION. 20
Page No 23
strictly involuntary muscles, as well as those which are least under the separate control of the will, are liable
still to act; and their action is often highly expressive. Conversely, when the will is temporarily or
permanently weakened, the voluntary muscles fail before the involuntary. It is a fact familiar to pathologists,
as Sir C. Bell remarks,[20] "that when debility arises from affection of the brain, the influence is greatest on
those muscles which are, in their natural condition, most under the command of the will." We shall, also, in
our future chapters, consider another proposition included in our first Principle; namely, that the checking of
one habitual movement sometimes requires other slight movements; these latter serving as a means of
expression.
[19] See the account given by this excellent observer in `Wild Sports of the Highlands,' 1846, p. 142.
[20] `Philosophical Translations,' 1823, p. 182.
CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_continued_.
The Principle of AntithesisInstances in the dog and cat Origin of the principleConventional
signsThe principle of antithesis has not arisen from opposite actions being consciously performed under
opposite impulses.
WE will now consider our second Principle, that of Antithesis. Certain states of the mind lead, as we have
seen in the last chapter, to certain habitual movements which were primarily, or may still be, of service; and
we shall find that when a directly opposite state of mind is induced, there is a strong and involuntary
tendency to the performance of movements of a directly opposite nature, though these have never been of any
service. A few striking instances of antithesis will be given, when we treat of the special expressions of man;
but as, in these cases, we are particularly liable to confound conventional or artificial gestures and
expressions with those which are innate or universal, and which alone deserve to rank as true expressions, I
will in the present chapter almost confine myself to the lower animals.
When a dog approaches a strange dog or man in a savage or hostile frame of mind be walks upright and very
stiffly; his head is slightly raised, or not much lowered; the tail is held erect, and quite rigid; the hairs bristle,
especially along the neck and back; the pricked ears are directed forwards, and the eyes have a fixed stare:
(see figs. 5 and 7). These actions, as will hereafter be explained, follow from the dog's intention to attack his
enemy, and are thus to a large extent intelligible. As he prepares to spring with a savage growl on his enemy,
the canine teeth are uncovered, and the ears are pressed close backwards on the head; but with these latter
actions, we are not here concerned. Let us now suppose that the dog suddenly discovers that the man he is
approaching, is not a stranger, but his master; and let it be observed how completely and instantaneously his
whole bearing is reversed. Instead of walking upright, the body sinks downwards or even crouches, and is
thrown into flexuous movements; his tail, instead of being held stiff and upright, is lowered and wagged from
side to side; his hair instantly becomes smooth; his ears are depressed and drawn backwards, but not closely
to the head; and his lips hang loosely. From the drawing back of the ears, the eyelids become elongated, and
the eyes no longer appear round and staring. It should be added that the animal is at such times in an excited
condition from joy; and nerveforce will be generated in excess, which naturally leads to action of some
kind. Not one of the above movements, so clearly expressive of affection, are of the least direct service to the
animal. They are explicable, as far as I can see, solely from being in complete opposition or antithesis to the
attitude and movements which, from intelligible causes, are assumed when a dog intends to fight, and which
consequently are expressive of anger. I request the reader to look at the four accompanying sketches, which
have been given in order to recall vividly the appearance of a dog under these two states of mind. It is,
however, not a little difficult to represent affection in a dog, whilst caressing his master and wagging his tail,
as the essence of
the expression lies in the continuous flexuous movements.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_continued_. 21
Page No 24
We will now turn to the cat. When this animal is threatened by a dog, it arches its back in a surprising
manner, erects its hair, opens its mouth and spits. But we are not here concerned with this wellknown
attitude, expressive of terror combined with anger; we are concerned only with that of rage or anger. This is
not often seen, but may be observed when two cats are fighting together; and I have seen it well exhibited by
a savage cat whilst plagued by a boy. The attitude is almost exactly the same as that of a tiger disturbed and
growling over its food, which every one must have beheld in menageries. The animal assumes a crouching
position, with the body extended; and the whole tail, or the tip alone, is lashed or curled from side to side.
The hair is not in the least erect. Thus far, the attitude and movements are nearly the same as when the animal
is prepared to spring on its prey, and when, no doubt, it feels savage. But when preparing to fight, there is this
difference, that the ears are closely pressed backwards; the mouth is partially opened, showing the teeth; the
fore feet are occasionally struck out with protruded claws; and the animal occasionally utters a fierce growl.
(See figs. 9 and 10.) All, or almost all these actions naturally follow (as hereafter to be explained), from the
cat's manner and intention of attacking its enemy.
Let us now look at a cat in a directly opposite frame of mind, whilst feeling affectionate and caressing her
master; and mark how opposite is her attitude in every respect. She now stands upright with her back slightly
arched, which makes the hair appear rather rough, but it does not bristle; her tail, instead of being extended
and lashed from side to side, is held quite still and perpendicularly upwards; her ears are erect and pointed;
her mouth is closed; and she rubs against her master with a purr instead of a growl. Let it further be observed
how widely different is the whole bearing of an affectionate cat from that of a dog, when with his body
crouching and flexuous, his tail lowered and wagging, and ears depressed, he caresses his master. This
contrast in the attitudes and movements of these two carnivorous animals, under the same pleased and
affectionate frame of mind, can be explained, as it appears to me, solely by their movements standing in
complete antithesis to those which are naturally assumed, when these animals feel savage and are prepared
either to fight or to seize their prey.
In these cases of the dog and cat, there is every reason to believe that the gestures both of hostility and
affection are innate or inherited; for they are almost identically the same in the different races of the species,
and in all the individuals of the same race, both young and old.
I will here give one other instance of antithesis in expression. I formerly possessed a large dog, who, like
every other dog, was much pleased to go out walking. He showed his pleasure by trotting gravely before me
with high steps, head much raised, moderately erected ears, and tail carried aloft but not stiffly. Not far from
my house a path branches off to the right, leading to the hothouse, which I used often to visit for a few
moments, to look at my experimental plants. This was always a great disappointment to the dog, as he did not
know whether I should continue my walk; and the instantaneous and complete change of expression which
came over him as soon as my body swerved in the least towards the path (and I sometimes tried this as an
experiment) was laughable. His look of dejection was known to every member of the family, and was called
his _hothouse face_. This consisted in the head drooping much, the whole body sinking a little and
remaining motionless; the ears and tail falling suddenly down, but the tail was by no means wagged. With the
falling of the ears and of his great chaps, the eyes became much changed in appearance, and I fancied that
they looked less bright. His aspect was that of piteous, hopeless dejection; and it was, as I have said,
laughable, as the cause was so slight. Every detail in his attitude was in complete opposition to his former
joyful yet dignified bearing; and can be explained, as it appears to me, in no other way, except through the
principle of antithesis. Had not the change been so instantaneous, I should have attributed it to his lowered
spirits affecting, as in the case of man, the nervous system and circulation, and consequently the tone of his
whole muscular frame; and this may have been in part the cause.
We will now consider how the principle of antithesis in expression has arisen. With social animals, the power
of intercommunication between the members of the same community,and with other species, between the
opposite sexes, as well as between the young and the old, is of the highest importance to them. This is
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_continued_. 22
Page No 25
generally effected by means of the voice, but it is certain that gestures and expressions are to a certain extent
mutually intelligible. Man not only uses inarticulate cries, gestures, and expressions, but has invented
articulate language; if, indeed, the word INVENTED can be applied to a process, completed by innumerable
steps, halfconsciously made. Any one who has watched monkeys will not doubt that they perfectly
understand each other's gestures and expression, and to a large extent, as Rengger asserts,[1] those of man.
An animal when going to attack another, or when afraid of another, often makes itself appear terrible, by
erecting its hair, thus increasing the apparent bulk of its body, by showing its teeth, or brandishing its horns,
or by uttering fierce sounds.
As the power of intercommunication is certainly of high service to many animals, there is no _a priori_
improbability in the supposition, that gestures manifestly of an opposite nature to those by which certain
feelings are already expressed, should at first have been voluntarily employed under the influence of an
opposite state of feeling. The fact of the gestures being now innate, would be no valid objection to the belief
that they were at first intentional; for if practised during many generations, they would probably at last be
inherited. Nevertheless it is more than doubtful, as we shall immediately see, whether any of the cases which
come under our present head of antithesis, have thus originated.
With conventional signs which are not innate, such as those used by the deaf and dumb and by savages, the
principle of opposition or antithesis has been partially brought into play. The Cistercian monks thought it
sinful to speak, and as they could not avoid holding some communication, they invented a gesture language,
in which the principle of opposition seems to have been employed.[2] Dr. Scott, of the Exeter Deaf and
Dumb Institution, writes to me that "opposites are greatly used in teaching the deaf and dumb, who have a
lively sense of them." Nevertheless I have been surprised how few unequivocal instances can be adduced.
This depends partly on all the signs having commonly had some natural origin; and partly on the practice of
the deaf and dumb and of savages to contract their signs as much as possible for the sake of rapidity?[3]
Hence their natural source or origin often becomes doubtful or is completely lost; as is likewise the case with
articulate language.
[1] `Naturgeschichte der Saugethiere von Paraguay,' 1830, s. 55.
[2] Mr. Tylor gives an account of the Cistercian gesturelanguage in his `Early History of Mankind' (2nd
edit. 1870, p. 40), and makes some remarks on the principle of opposition in gestures.
[3] See on this subject Dr. W. R. Scott's interesting work, `The Deaf and Dumb,' 2nd edit. 1870, p. 12. He
says, "This contracting of natural gestures into much shorter gestures than the natural expression requires, is
very common amongst the deaf and dumb. This contracted gesture is frequently so shortened as nearly to lose
all semblance of the natural one, but to the deaf and dumb who use it, it still has the force of the original
expression."
Many signs, moreover, which plainly stand in opposition to each other, appear to have had on both sides a
significant origin. This seems to hold good with the signs used by the deal and dumb for light and darkness,
for strength and weakness, In a future chapter I shall endeavour to show that the opposite gestures of
affirmation and negation, namely, vertically nodding and laterally shaking the head, have both probably had a
natural beginning. The waving of the hand from right to left, which is used as a negative by some savages,
may have been invented in imitation of shaking the head; but whether the opposite movement of waving the
hand in a straight line from the face, which is used in affirmation, has arisen through antithesis or in some
quite distinct manner, is doubtful.
If we now turn to the gestures which are innate or common to all the individuals of the same species, and
which come under the present head of antithesis, it is extremely doubtful, whether any of them were at first
deliberately invented and consciously performed. With mankind the best instance of a gesture standing in
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_continued_. 23
Page No 26
direct opposition to other movements, naturally assumed under an opposite frame of mind, is that of
shrugging the shoulders. This expresses impotence or an apology,something which cannot be done, or
cannot be avoided. The gesture is sometimes used consciously and voluntarily, but it is extremely improbable
that it was at first deliberately invented, and afterwards fixed by habit; for not only do young children
sometimes shrug their shoulders under the above states of mind, but the movement is accompanied, as will be
shown in a future chapter, by various subordinate movements, which not one man in a thousand is aware of,
unless he has specially attended to the subject.
Dogs when approaching a strange dog, may find it useful to show by their movements that they are friendly,
and do not wish to fight. When two young dogs in play are growling and biting each other's faces and legs, it
is obvious that they mutually understand each other's gestures and manners. There seems, indeed, some
degree of instinctive knowledge in puppies and kittens, that they must not use their sharp little teeth or claws
too freely in their play, though this sometimes happens and a squeal is the result; otherwise they would often
injure each other's eyes. When my terrier bites my hand in play, often snarling at the same time, if he bites
too hard and I say GENTLY, GENTLY, he goes on biting, but answers me by a few wags of the tail, which
seems to say "Never mind, it is all fun." Although dogs do thus express, and may wish to express, to other
dogs and to man, that they are in a friendly state of mind, it is incredible that they could ever have
deliberately thought of drawing back and depressing their ears, instead of holding them erect,of lowering
and wagging their tails, instead of keeping them stiff and upright, because they knew that these movements
stood in direct opposition to those assumed under an opposite and savage frame of mind.
Again, when a cat, or rather when some early progenitor of the species, from feeling affectionate first slightly
arched its back, held its tail perpendicularly upwards and pricked its ears, can it be believed that the animal
consciously wished thus to show that its frame of mind was directly the reverse of that, when from being
ready to fight or to spring on its prey, it assumed a crouching attitude, curled its tail from side to side and
depressed its ears? Even still less can I believe that my dog voluntarily put on his dejected attitude and
"_hothouse face_," which formed so complete a contrast to his previous cheerful attitude and whole bearing.
It cannot be supposed that he knew that I should understand his expression, and that he could thus soften my
heart and make me give up visiting the hothouse.
Hence for the development of the movements which come under the present head, some other principle,
distinct from the will and consciousness, must have intervened. This principle appears to be that every
movement which we have voluntarily performed throughout our lives has required the action of certain
muscles; and when we have performed a directly opposite movement, an opposite set of muscles has been
habitually brought into play, as in turning to the right or to the left, in pushing away or pulling an object
towards us, and in lifting or lowering a weight. So strongly are our intentions and movements associated
together, that if we eagerly wish an object to move in any direction, we can hardly avoid moving our bodies
in the same direction, although we may be perfectly aware that this can have no influence. A good illustration
of this fact has already been given in the Introduction, namely, in the grotesque movements of a young and
eager billiardplayer, whilst watching the course of his ball. A man or child in a passion, if he tells any one in
a loud voice to begone, generally moves his arm as if to push him away, although the offender may not be
standing near, and although there may be not the least need to explain by a gesture what is meant. On the
other hand, if we eagerly desire some one to approach us closely, we act as if pulling him towards us; and so
in innumerable other instances.
As the performance of ordinary movements of an opposite kind, under opposite impulses of the will, has
become habitual in us and in the lower animals, so when actions of one kind have become firmly associated
with any sensation or emotion, it appears natural that actions of a directly opposite kind, though of no use,
should be unconsciously performed through habit and association, under the influence of a directly opposite
sensation or emotion. On this principle alone can I understand how the gestures and expressions which come
under the present head of antithesis have originated. If indeed they are serviceable to man or to any other
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_continued_. 24
Page No 27
animal, in aid of inarticulate cries or language, they will likewise be voluntarily employed, and the habit will
thus be strengthened. But whether or not of service as a means of communication, the tendency to perform
opposite movements under opposite sensations or emotions would, if we may judge by analogy, become
hereditary through long practice; and there cannot be a doubt that several expressive movements due to the
principle of antithesis are inherited.
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_.
The principle of direct action of the excited nervous system on the body, independently of the will and in part
of habit Change of colour in the hairTrembling of the muscles Modified
secretionsPerspirationExpression of extreme pain Of rage, great joy, and terrorContrast between
the emotions which cause and do not cause expressive movementsExciting and depressing states of the
mindSummary.
WE now come to our third Principle, namely, that certain actions which we recognize as expressive of certain
states of the mind, are the direct result of the constitution of the nervous system, and have been from the first
independent of the will, and, to a large extent, of habit. When the sensorium is strongly excited nerveforce is
generated in excess, and is transmitted in certain directions, dependent on the connection of the nervecells,
and, as far as the muscular system is concerned, on the nature of the movements which have been habitually
practised. Or the supply of nerveforce may, as it appears, be interrupted. Of course every movement which
we make is determined by the constitution of the nervous system; but actions performed in obedience to the
will, or through habit, or through the principle of antithesis, are here as far as possible excluded. Our present
subject is very obscure, but, from its importance, must be discussed at some little length; and it is always
advisable to perceive clearly our ignorance.
The most striking case, though a rare and abnormal one, which can be adduced of the direct influence of the
nervous system, when strongly affected, on the body, is the loss of colour in the hair, which has occasionally
been observed after extreme terror or grief. One authentic instance has been recorded, in the case of a man
brought out for execution in India, in which the change of colour was so rapid that it was perceptible to the
eye.[1]
Another good case is that of the trembling of the muscles, which is common to man and to many, or most, of
the lower animals. Trembling is of no service, often of much disservice, and cannot have been at first
acquired through the will, and then rendered habitual in association with any emotion. I am assured by an
eminent authority that young children do not tremble, but go into convulsions under the circumstances which
would induce excessive trembling in adults. Trembling is excited in different individuals in very different
degrees. and by the most diversified causes,by cold to the surface, before feverfits, although the
temperature of the body is then above the normal standard; in bloodpoisoning, delirium tremens, and other
diseases; by general failure of power in old age; by exhaustion after excessive fatigue; locally from severe
injuries, such as burns; and, in an especial manner, by the passage of a catheter. Of all emotions, fear
notoriously is the most apt to induce trembling; but so do occasionally great anger and joy. I remember once
seeing a boy who had just shot his first snipe on the wing, and his hands
[1] See the interesting cases collected by M. G. Pouchet in the `Revue des Deux Mondes,' January 1, 1872, p.
79. An instance was also brought some years ago before the British Association at Belfast. trembled to such a
degree from delight, that he could not for some time reload his gun; and I have heard of an exactly similar
case with an Australian savage, to whom a gun had been lent. Fine music, from the vague emotions thus
excited, causes a shiver to run down the backs of some persons. There seems to be very little in common in
the above several physical causes and emotions to account for trembling; and Sir J. Paget, to whom I am
indebted for several of the above statements, informs me that the subject is a very obscure one. As trembling
is sometimes caused by rage, long before exhaustion can have set in, and as it sometimes accompanies great
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. 25
Page No 28
joy, it would appear that any strong excitement of the nervous system interrupts the steady flow of
nerveforce to the muscles.[2]
The manner in which the secretions of the alimentary canal and of certain glandsas the liver, kidneys, or
mammae are affected by strong emotions, is another excellent instance of the direct action of the sensorium
on these organs, independently of the will or of any serviceable associated habit. There is the greatest
difference in different persons in the parts which are thus affected, and in the degree of their affection.
The heart, which goes on uninterruptedly beating night and day in so wonderful a manner, is extremely
sensitive to external stimulants. The great physiologist, Claude Bernard,[3] has shown bow the least
excitement of a sensitive nerve reacts on the heart; even when a nerve is touched so slightly that no pain can
possibly be felt by the animal under experiment. Hence when the mind is strongly excited, we might expect
that it would instantly affect in a direct manner the heart; and this is universally acknowledged and felt to be
the case. Claude Bernard also repeatedly insists, and this deserves especial notice, that when the heart is
affected it reacts on the brain; and the state of the brain again reacts through the pneumogastric nerve on the
heart; so that under any excitement there will be much mutual action and reaction between these, the two
most important organs of the body.
[2] Muller remarks (`Elements of Physiology,' Eng. translat. vol. ii. p. 934) that when the feelings are very
intense, "all the spinal nerves become affected to the extent of imperfect paralysis, or the excitement of
trembling of the whole body."
[3] `Lecons sur les Prop. des Tissus Vivants,' 1866, pp. 457466.
The vasomotor system, which regulates the diameter of the small arteries, is directly acted on by the
sensorium, as we see when a man blushes from shame; but in this latter case the checked transmission of
nerveforce to the vessels of the face can, I think, be partly explained in a curious manner through habit. We
shall also be able to throw some light, though very little, on the involuntary erection of the hair under the
emotions of terror and rage. The secretion of tears depends, no doubt, on the connection of certain
nervecells; but here again we can trace some few of the steps by which the flow of nerveforce through the
requisite channels has become habitual under certain emotions.
A brief consideration of the outward signs of some of the stronger sensations and emotions will best serve to
show us, although vaguely, in how complex a manner the principle under consideration of the direct action of
the excited nervous system of the body, is combined with the principle of habitually associated, serviceable
movements.
When animals suffer from an agony of pain, they generally writhe about with frightful contortions; and those
which habitually use their voices utter piercing cries or groans. Almost every muscle of the body is brought
into strong action. With man the mouth may be closely compressed, or more commonly the lips are retracted,
with the teeth clenched or ground together. There is said to be "gnashing of teeth" in hell; and I have plainly
heard the grinding of the molar teeth of a cow which was suffering acutely from inflammation of the bowels.
The female hippopotamus in the Zoological Gardens, when she produced her young, suffered greatly; she
incessantly walked about, or rolled on her sides, opening and closing her jaws, and clattering her teeth
together.[4] With man the eyes stare wildly as in horrified astonishment, or the brows are heavily contracted.
Perspiration bathes the body, and drops trickle down the face. The circulation and respiration are much
affected. Hence the nostrils are generally dilated and often quiver; or the breath may be held until the blood
stagnates in the purple face. If the agony be severe and prolonged, these signs all change; utter prostration
follows, with fainting or convulsions.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. 26
Page No 29
A sensitive nerve when irritated transmits some influence to the nervecell, whence it proceeds; and this
transmits its influence, first to the corresponding nervecell on the opposite side of the body, and then
upwards and downwards along the cerebrospinal column to other nervecells, to a greater or less extent,
according to the strength of the excitement; so that, ultimately, the whole nervous system maybe affected.[5]
This involuntary transmission of nerveforce may or may not be accompanied by consciousness. Why the
irritation of a nervecell should generate or liberate nerveforce is not known; but that this is the case seems
to be the conclusion arrived at by all the greatest physiologists, such as Muller, Virchow, Bernard, As Mr.
Herbert Spencer remarks, it may be received as an "unquestionable truth that, at any moment, the existing
quantity of liberated nerveforce, which in an inscrutable way produces in us the state we call feeling, MUST
expend itself in some directionMUST generate an equivalent manifestation of force somewhere;" so that,
when the cerebrospinal system is highly excited and nerveforce is liberated in excess, it may be expended
in intense sensations, active thought, violent movements, or increased activity of the glands.[7] Mr. Spencer
further maintains that an "overflow of nerveforce, undirected by any motive, will manifestly take the most
habitual routes; and, if these do not suffice, will next overflow into the less habitual ones." Consequently the
facial and respiratory muscles, which are the most used, will be apt to be first brought into action; then those
of the upper extremities, next those of the lower, and finally those of the whole body.[8]
[4] Mr. Bartlett, "Notes on the Birth of a Hippopotamus," Proc. Zoolog. Soc. 1871, p. 255.
[5] See, on this subject, Claude Bernard, `Tissus Vivants,' 1866, pp. 316, 337, 358. Virchow expresses
himself to almost exactly the same effect in his essay "Ueber das Ruckenmark" (Sammlung wissenschaft.
Vortrage, 1871, s. 28).
An emotion may be very strong, but it will have little tendency to induce movements of any kind, if it has not
commonly led to voluntary action for its relief or gratification; and when movements are excited, their nature
is, to a large extent, determined by those which have often and voluntarily been performed for some definite
end under the same emotion. Great pain urges all animals, and has urged them during endless generations, to
make the most violent and diversified efforts to escape from the cause of suffering. Even when a limb or
other separate part of the body is hurt, we often see a tendency to shake it, as if to shake off the cause, though
this may obviously be impossible. Thus a habit of exerting with the utmost force all the muscles will have
been established, whenever great suffering is experienced. As the muscles of the chest and vocal organs are
habitually used, these will be particularly liable to be acted on, and loud, harsh screams or cries will be
uttered. But the advantage derived from outcries has here probably come into play in an important manner;
for the young of most animals, when in distress or danger, call loudly to their parents for aid, as do the
members of the same community for mutual aid.
[6] Muller (`Elements of Physiology,' Eng. translat. vol. ii. p. 932) in speaking of the nerves, says, "any
sudden change of condition of whatever kind sets the nervous principle into action." See Virchow and
Bernard on the same subject in passages in the two works referred to in my last footnote.
[7] H. Spencer, `Essays, Scientific, Political,' Second Series, 1863, pp. 109, 111.
[8] Sir H. Holland, in speaking (`Medical Notes and Reflexions,' 1839, p. 328) of that curious state of body
called the _fidgets_, remarks that it seems due to "an accumulation of some cause of irritation which requires
muscular action for its relief."
Another principle, namely, the internal consciousness that the power or capacity of the nervous system is
limited, will have strengthened, though in a subordinate degree, the tendency to violent action under extreme
suffering. A man cannot think deeply and exert his utmost muscular force. As Hippocrates long ago observed,
if two pains are felt at the same time, the severer one dulls the other. Martyrs, in the ecstasy of their religious
fervour have often, as it would appear, been insensible to the most horrid tortures. Sailors who are going to be
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. 27
Page No 30
flogged sometimes take a piece of lead into their mouths, in order to bite it with their utmost force, and thus
to bear the pain. Parturient women prepare to exert their muscles to the utmost in order to relieve their
sufferings.
We thus see that the undirected radiation of nerveforce from the nervecells which are first affectedthe
longcontinued habit of attempting by struggling to escape from the cause of suffering and the
consciousness that voluntary muscular exertion relieves pain, have all probably concurred in giving a
tendency to the most violent, almost convulsive, movements under extreme suffering; and such movements,
including those of the vocal organs, are universally recognized as highly expressive of this condition.
As the mere touching of a sensitive nerve reacts in a direct manner on the heart, severe pain will obviously
react on it in like manner, but far more energetically. Nevertheless, even in this case, we must not overlook
the indirect effects of habit on the heart, as we shall see when we consider the signs of rage.
When a man suffers from an agony of pain, the perspiration often trickles down his face; and I have been
assured by a veterinary surgeon that he has frequently seen drops falling from the belly and running down the
inside of the thighs of horses, and from the bodies of cattle, when thus suffering. He has observed this, when
there has been no struggling which would account for the perspiration. The whole body of the female
hippopotamus, before alluded to, was covered with redcoloured perspiration whilst giving birth to her
young. So it is with extreme fear; the same veterinary has often seen horses sweating from this cause; as has
Mr. Bartlett with the rhinoceros; and with man it is a wellknown symptom. The cause of perspiration
bursting forth in these cases is quite obscure; but it is thought by some physiologists to be connected with the
failing power of the capillary circulation; and we know that the vasomotor system, which regulates the
capillary circulation, is much influenced by the mind. With respect to the movements of certain muscles of
the face under great suffering, as well as from other emotions, these will be best considered when we treat of
the special expressions of man and of the lower animals.
We will now turn to the characteristic symptoms of Rage. Under this powerful emotion the action of the heart
is much accelerated,[9] or it may be much disturbed. The face reddens, or it becomes purple from the
impeded return of the blood, or may turn deadly pale. The respiration is laboured, the chest heaves, and the
dilated nostrils quiver. The whole body often trembles. The voice is affected. The teeth are clenched or
ground together, and the muscular system is commonly stimulated to violent, almost frantic action. But the
gestures of a man in this state usually differ from the purposeless writhings and struggles of one suffering
from an agony of pain; for they represent more or less plainly the act of striking or fighting with an enemy.
All these signs of rage are probably in large part, and some of them appear to be wholly, due to the direct
action of the excited sensorium. But animals of all kinds, and their progenitors before them, when attacked or
threatened by an enemy, have exerted their utmost powers in fighting and in defending themselves. Unless an
animal does thus act, or has the intention, or at least the desire, to attack its enemy, it cannot properly be said
to be enraged. An inherited habit of muscular exertion will thus have been gained in association with rage;
and this will directly or indirectly affect various organs, in nearly the same manner as does great bodily
suffering.
[9] I am much indebted to Mr. A. H. Garrod for having informed me of M. Lorain's work on the pulse, in
which a sphygmogram of a woman in a rage is given; and this shows much difference in the rate and other
characters from that of the same woman in her ordinary state.
The heart no doubt will likewise be affected in a direct manner; but it will also in all probability be affected
through habit; and all the more so from not being under the control of the will. We know that any great
exertion which we voluntarily make, affects the heart, through mechanical and other principles which need
not here be considered; and it was shown in the first chapter that nerveforce flows readily through habitually
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. 28
Page No 31
used channels,through the nerves of voluntary or involuntary movement, and through those of sensation.
Thus even a moderate amount of exertion will tend to act on the heart; and on the principle of association, of
which so many instances have been given, we may feel nearly sure that any sensation or emotion, as great
pain or rage, which has habitually led to much muscular action, will immediately influence the flow of
nerveforce to the heart, although there may not be at the time any muscular exertion.
The heart, as I have said, will be all the more readily affected through habitual associations, as it is not under
the control of the will. A man when moderately angry, or even when enraged, may command the movements
of his body, but he cannot prevent his heart from beating rapidly. His chest will perhaps give a few heaves,
and his nostrils just quiver, for the movements of respiration are only in part voluntary. In like manner those
muscles of the face which are least obedient to the will, will sometimes alone betray a slight and passing
emotion. The glands again are wholly independent of the will, and a man suffering from grief may command
his features, but cannot always prevent the tears from coming into his eyes. A hungry man, if tempting food
is placed before him, may not show his hunger by any outward gesture, but he cannot check the secretion of
saliva.
Under a transport of Joy or of vivid Pleasure, there is a strong tendency to various purposeless movements,
and to the utterance of various sounds. We see this in our young children, in their loud laughter, clapping of
hands, and jumping for joy; in the bounding and barking of a dog when going out to walk with his master;
and in the frisking of a horse when turned out into an open field. Joy quickens the circulation, and this
stimulates the brain, which again reacts on the whole body. The above purposeless movements and increased
heartaction may be attributed in chief part to the excited state of the sensorium,[10] and to the consequent
undirected overflow, as Mr. Herbert Spencer insists, of nerveforce. It deserves notice, that it is chiefly the
anticipation of a pleasure, and not its actual enjoyment, which leads to purposeless and extravagant
movements of the body, and to the utterance of various sounds. We see this in our children when they expect
any great pleasure or treat; and dogs, which have been bounding about at
[10] How powerfully intense joy excites the brain, and how the brain reacts on the body, is well shown in the
rare cases of Psychical Intoxication. Dr. J. Crichton Browne (`Medical Mirror,' 1865) records the case of a
young man of strongly nervous temperament, who, on hearing by a telegram that a fortune had been
bequeathed him, first became pale, then exhilarated, and soon in the highest spirits, but flushed and very
restless. He then took a walk with a friend for the sake of tranquillising himself, but returned staggering in his
gait, uproariously laughing, yet irritable in temper, incessantly talking, and singing loudly in the public
streets. It was positively ascertained that he had not touched any spirituous liquor, though every one thought
that he was intoxicated. Vomiting after a time came on, and the halfdigested contents of his stomach were
examined, but no odour of alcohol could be detected. He then slept heavily, and on awaking was well, except
that he suffered from headache, nausea, and prostration of strength. the sight of a plate of food, when they get
it do not show their delight by any outward sign, not even by wagging their tails. Now with animals of all
kinds, the acquirement of almost all their pleasures, with the exception of those of warmth and rest, are
associated, and have long been associated with active movements, as in the hunting or search for food, and in
their courtship. Moreover, the mere exertion of the muscles after long rest or confinement is in itself a
pleasure, as we ourselves feel, and as we see in the play of young animals. Therefore on this latter principle
alone we might perhaps expect, that vivid pleasure would be apt to show itself conversely in muscular
movements.
With all or almost all animals, even with birds, Terror causes the body to tremble. The skin becomes pale,
sweat breaks out, and the hair bristles. The secretions of the alimentary canal and of the kidneys are
increased, and they are involuntarily voided, owing to the relaxation of the sphincter muscles, as is known to
be the case with man, and as I have seen with cattle, dogs, cats, and monkeys. The breathing is hurried. The
heart beats quickly, wildly, and violently; but whether it pumps the blood more efficiently through the body
may be doubted, for the surface seems bloodless and the strength of the muscles soon fails. In a frightened
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. 29
Page No 32
horse I have felt through the saddle the beating of the heart so plainly that I could have counted the beats. The
mental faculties are much disturbed. Utter prostration soon follows, and even fainting. A terrified
canarybird has been seen not only to tremble and to turn white about the base of the bill, but to faint;[11]
and I once caught a robin in a room, which fainted so completely, that for a time I thought it dead.
[11] Dr. Darwin, `Zoonomia,' 1794, vol. i. p. 148.
Most of these symptoms are probably the direct result, independently of habit, of the disturbed state of the
sensorium; but it is doubtful whether they ought to be wholly thus accounted for. When an animal is alarmed
it almost always stands motionless for a moment, in order to collect its senses and to ascertain the source of
danger, and sometimes for the sake of escaping detection. But headlong flight soon follows, with no
husbanding of the strength as in fighting, and the animal continues to fly as long as the danger lasts, until
utter prostration, with failing respiration and circulation, with all the muscles quivering and profuse sweating,
renders further flight impossible. Hence it does not seem improbable that the principle of associated habit
may in part account for, or at least augment, some of the abovenamed characteristic symptoms of extreme
terror.
That the principle of associated habit has played an important part in causing the movements expressive of
the foregoing several strong emotions and sensations, we may, I think, conclude from considering firstly,
some other strong emotions which do not ordinarily require for their relief or gratification any voluntary
movement; and secondly the contrast in nature between the socalled exciting and depressing states of the
mind. No emotion is stronger than maternal love; but a mother may feel the deepest love for her helpless
infant, and yet not show it by any outward sign; or only by slight caressing movements, with a gentle smile
and tender eyes. But let any one intentionally injure her infant, and see what a change! how she starts up with
threatening aspect, how her eyes sparkle and her face reddens, how her bosom heaves, nostrils dilate, and
heart beats; for anger, and not maternal love, has habitually led to action. The love between the opposite
sexes is widely different from maternal love; and when lovers meet, we know that their hearts beat quickly,
their breathing is hurried, and their faces flush; for this love is not inactive like that of a mother for her infant.
A man may have his mind filled with the blackest hatred or suspicion, or be corroded with envy or jealousy,
but as these feelings do not at once lead to action, and as they commonly last for some time, they are not
shown by any outward sign, excepting that a man in this state assuredly does not appear cheerful or
goodtempered. If indeed these feelings break out into overt acts, rage takes their place, and will be plainly
exhibited. Painters can hardly portray suspicion, jealousy, envy, except by the aid of accessories which tell
the tale; and poets use such vague and fanciful expressions as "greeneyed jealousy." Spenser describes
suspicion as "Foul, illfavoured, and grim, under his eyebrows looking still askance," Shakespeare speaks of
envy "as leanfaced in her loathsome case;" and in another place he says, "no black envy shall make my
grave;" and again as "above pale envy's threatening reach."
Emotions and sensations have often been classed as exciting or depressing. When all the organs of the body
and mind,those of voluntary and involuntary movement, of perception, sensation, thought, their functions
more energetically and rapidly than usual, a man or animal may be said to be excited, and, under an opposite
state, to be depressed. Anger and joy are from the first exciting emotions, and they naturally lead, more
especially the former, to energetic movements, which react on the heart and this again on the brain. A
physician once remarked to me as a proof of the exciting nature of anger, that a man when excessively jaded
will sometimes invent imaginary offences and put himself into a passion, unconsciously for the sake of
reinvigorating himself; and since hearing this remark, I have occasionally recognized its full truth.
Several other states of mind appear to be at first exciting, but soon become depressing to an extreme degree.
When a mother suddenly loses her child, sometimes she is frantic with grief, and must be considered to be in
an excited state; she walks wildly about, tears her hair or clothes, and wrings her hands. This latter action is
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. 30
Page No 33
perhaps due to the principle of antithesis, betraying an inward sense of helplessness and that nothing can be
done. The other wild and violent movements may be in part explained by the relief experienced through
muscular exertion, and in part by the undirected overflow of nerveforce from the excited sensorium. But
under the sudden loss of a beloved person, one of the first and commonest thoughts which occurs, is that
something more might have been done to save the lost one. An excellent observer,[12] in describing the
behaviour of a girl at the sudden death of her father, says she "went about the house wringing her hands like a
creature demented, saying `It was her fault;' `I should never have left him;' `If I had only sat up with him,' "
With such ideas vividly present before the mind, there would arise, through the principle of associated habit,
the strongest tendency to energetic action of some kind.
As soon as the sufferer is fully conscious that nothing can be done, despair or deep sorrow takes the place of
frantic grief. The sufferer sits motionless, or gently rocks to and fro; the circulation becomes languid;
respiration is almost forgotten, and deep sighs are drawn.
[12] "Mrs. Oliphant, in her novel of `Miss Majoribanks,' p. 362. All this reacts on the brain, and prostration
soon follows with collapsed muscles and dulled eyes. As associated habit no longer prompts the sufferer to
action, he is urged by his friends to voluntary exertion, and not to give way to silent, motionless grief.
Exertion stimulates the heart, and this reacts on the brain, and aids the mind to bear its heavy load.
Pain, if severe, soon induces extreme depression or prostration; but it is at first a stimulant and excites to
action, as we see when we whip a horse, and as is shown by the horrid tortures inflicted in foreign lands on
exhausted draybullocks, to rouse them to renewed exertion. Fear again is the most depressing of all the
emotions; and it soon induces utter, helpless prostration, as if in consequence of, or in association with, the
most violent and prolonged attempts to escape from the danger, though no such attempts have actually been
made. Nevertheless, even extreme fear often acts at first as a powerful stimulant. A man or animal driven
through terror to desperation, is endowed with wonderful strength, and is notoriously dangerous in the
highest degree.
On the whole we may conclude that the principle of the direct action of the sensorium on the body, due to the
constitution of the nervous system, and from the first independent of the will, has been highly influential in
determining many expressions. Good instances are afforded by the trembling of the muscles, the sweating of
the skin, the modified secretions of the alimentary canal and glands, under various emotions and sensations.
But actions of this kind are often combined with others, which follow from our first principle, namely, that
actions which have often been of direct or indirect service, under certain states of the mind, in order to gratify
or relieve certain sensations, desires, are still performed under analogous circumstances through mere habit
although of no service. We have combinations of this kind, at least in part, in the frantic gestures of rage and
in the writhings of extreme pain; and, perhaps, in the increased action of the heart and of the respiratory
organs. Even when these and other emotions or sensations are aroused in a very feeble manner, there will still
be a tendency to similar actions, owing to the force of longassociated habit; and those actions which are
least under voluntary control will generally be longest retained. Our second principle of antithesis has
likewise occasionally come into play.
Finally, so many expressive movements can be explained, as I trust will be seen in the course of this volume,
through the three principles which have now been discussed, that we may hope hereafter to see all thus
explained, or by closely analogous principles. It is, however, often impossible to decide how much weight
ought to be attributed, in each particular case, to one of our principles, and how much to another; and very
many points in the theory of Expression remain inexplicable.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION_concluded_. 31
Page No 34
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS.
The emission of SoundsVocal soundsSounds otherwise produced Erection of the dermal appendages,
hairs, feathers, under the emotions of anger and terrorThe drawing back of the ears as a preparation for
fighting, and as an expression of anger Erection of the ears and raising the head, a sign of attention.
IN this and the following chapter I will describe, but only in sufficient detail to illustrate my subject, the
expressive movements, under different states of the mind, of some few wellknown animals. But before
considering them in due succession, it will save much useless repetition to discuss certain means of
expression common to most of them.
_The emission of Sounds_.With many kinds of animals, man included, the vocal organs are efficient in the
highest degree as a means of expression. We have seen, in the last chapter, that when the sensorium is
strongly excited, the muscles of the body are generally thrown into violent action; and as a consequence, loud
sounds are uttered, however silent the animal may generally be, and although the sounds may be of no use.
Hares and rabbits for instance, never, I believe, use their vocal organs except in the extremity of suffering; as,
when a wounded hare is killed by the sportsman, or when a young rabbit is caught by a stoat. Cattle and
horses suffer great pain in silence; but when this is excessive, and especially when associated with terror, they
utter fearful sounds. I have often recognized, from a distance on the Pampas, the agonized deathbellow of
the cattle, when caught by the lasso and hamstrung. It is said that horses, when attacked by wolves, utter loud
and peculiar screams of distress.
Involuntary and purposeless contractions of the muscles of the chest and glottis, excited in the above manner,
may have first given rise to the emission of vocal sounds. But the voice is now largely used by many animals
for various purposes; and habit seems to have played an important part in its employment under other
circumstances. Naturalists have remarked, I believe with truth, that social animals, from habitually using their
vocal organs as a means of intercommunication, use them on other occasions much more freely than other
animals. But there are marked exceptions to this rule, for instance, with the rabbit. The principle, also, of
association, which is so widely extended in its power, has likewise played its part. Hence it follows that the
voice, from having been habitually employed as a serviceable aid under certain conditions, inducing pleasure,
pain, rage, is commonly used whenever the same sensations or emotions are excited, under quite different
conditions, or in a lesser degree.
The sexes of many animals incessantly call for each other during the breedingseason; and in not a few cases,
the male endeavours thus to charm or excite the female. This, indeed, seems to have been the primeval use
and means of development of the voice, as I have attempted to show in my `Descent of Man.' Thus the use of
the vocal organs will have become associated with the anticipation of the strongest pleasure which animals
are capable of feeling. Animals which live in society often call to each other when separated, and evidently
feel much joy at meeting; as we see with a horse, on the return of his companion, for whom he has been
neighing. The mother calls incessantly for her lost young ones; for instance, a cow for her calf; and the young
of many animals call for their mothers. When a flock of sheep is scattered, the ewes bleat incessantly for their
lambs, and their mutual pleasure at coming together is manifest. Woe betide the man who meddles with the
young of the larger and fiercer quadrupeds, if they hear the cry of distress from their young. Rage leads to the
violent exertion of all the muscles, including those of the voice; and some animals, when enraged, endeavour
to strike terror into their enemies by its power and harshness, as the lion does by roaring, and the dog by
growling. I infer that their object is to strike terror, because the lion at the same time erects the hair of its
mane, and the dog the hair along its back, and thus they make themselves appear as large and terrible as
possible. Rival males try to excel and challenge each other by their voices, and this leads to deadly contests.
Thus the use of the voice will have become associated with the emotion of anger, however it may be aroused.
We have also seen that intense pain, like rage, leads to violent outcries, and the exertion of screaming by
itself gives some relief; and thus the use of the voice will have become associated with suffering of any kind.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 32
Page No 35
The cause of widely different sounds being uttered under different emotions and sensations is a very obscure
subject. Nor does the rule always hold good that there is any marked difference. For instance with the dog,
the bark of anger and that of joy do not differ much, though they can be distinguished. It is not probable that
any precise explanation of the cause or source of each particular sound, under different states of the mind,
will ever be given. We now that some animals, after being domesticated, have acquired the habit of uttering
sounds which were not natural to them.[1] Thus domestic dogs, and even tamed jackals, have learnt to bark,
which is a noise not proper to any species of the genus, with the exception of the _Canis latrans_ of North
America, which is said to bark. Some breeds, also, of the domestic pigeon have learnt to coo in a new and
quite peculiar manner.
The character of the human voice, under the influence of various emotions, has been discussed by Mr.
Herbert Spencer[2] in his interesting essay on Music. He clearly shows that the voice alters much under
different conditions, in loudness and in quality, that is, in resonance and _timbre_, in pitch and intervals. No
one can listen to an eloquent orator or preacher, or to a man calling angrily to another, or to one expressing
astonishment, without being struck with the truth of Mr. Spencer's remarks. It is curious how early in life the
modulation of the voice becomes expressive. With one of my children, under the age of two years, I clearly
perceived that his humph of assent was rendered by a slight modulation strongly emphatic; and that by a
peculiar whine his negative expressed obstinate determination. Mr. Spencer further shows that emotional
speech, in all the above respects is intimately related to vocal music, and consequently to instrumental music;
and he attempts to explain the characteristic qualities of both on physiological groundsnamely, on "the
general law that a feeling is a stimulus to muscular action." It may be admitted that the voice is affected
through this law; but the explanation appears to me too general and vague to throw much light on the various
differences, with the exception of that of loudness, between ordinary speech and emotional speech, or
singing.
[1] See the evidence on this head in my `Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,' vol. i. p. 27.
On the cooing of pigeons, vol. i. pp. 154, 155.
[2] `Essays, Scientific, Political, and Speculative,' 1858. `The Origin and Function of Music,' p. 359.
This remark holds good, whether we believe that the various qualities of the voice originated in speaking
under the excitement of strong feelings, and that these qualities have subsequently been transferred to vocal
music; or whether we believe, as I maintain, that the habit of uttering musical sounds was first developed, as
a means of courtship, in the early progenitors of man, and thus became associated with the strongest emotions
of which they were capable,namely, ardent love, rivalry and triumph. That animals utter musical notes is
familiar to every one, as we may daily hear in the singing of birds. It is a more remarkable fact that an ape,
one of the Gibbons, produces an exact octave of musical sounds, ascending and descending the scale by
halftones; so that this monkey "alone of brute mammals may be said to sing."[3] From this fact, and from the
analogy of other animals, I have been led to infer that the progenitors of man probably uttered musical tones,
before they had acquired the power of articulate speech; and that consequently, when the voice is used under
any strong emotion, it tends to assume, through the principle of association, a musical character. We can
plainly perceive, with some of the lower animals, that the males employ their voices to please the females,
and that they themselves take pleasure in their own vocal utterances; but why particular sounds are uttered,
and why these give pleasure cannot at present be explained.
[3] `The Descent of Man,' 1870, vol. ii. p. 332. The words quoted are from Professor Owen. It has lately been
shown that some quadrupeds much lower in the scale than monkeys, namely Rodents, are able to produce
correct musical tones: see the account of a singing Hesperomys, by the Rev. S. Lockwood, in the `American
Naturalist,' vol. v. December, 1871, p. 761.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 33
Page No 36
That the pitch of the voice bears some relation to certain states of feeling is tolerably clear. A person gently
complaining of illtreatment, or slightly suffering, almost always speaks in a highpitched voice. Dogs, when
a little impatient, often make a high piping note through their noses, which at once strikes us as plaintive;[4]
but how difficult it is to know whether the sound is essentially plaintive, or only appears so in this particular
case, from our having learnt by experience what it means! Rengger, states[5] that the monkeys (_Cebus
azaroe_), which he kept in Paraguay, expressed astonishment by a halfpiping, halfsnarling noise; anger or
impatience, by repeating the sound _hu hu_ in a deeper, grunting voice; and fright or pain, by shrill screams.
On the other hand, with mankind, deep groans and high piercing screams equally express an agony of pain.
Laughter maybe either high or low; so that, with adult men, as Haller long ago remarked,[6] the sound
partakes of the character of the vowels (as pronounced in German) _O_ and _A_; whilst with children and
women, it has more of the character of _E_ and _I_; and these latter vowelsounds naturally have, as
Helmholtz has shown, a higher pitch than the former; yet both tones of laughter equally express enjoyment or
amusement.
In considering the mode in which vocal utterances express emotion, we are naturally led to inquire into the
cause of what is called "expression" in music. Upon this point Mr. Litchfield, who has long attended to the
subject of music, has been so kind as to give me the following remarks:"The question, what is the essence
of musical `expression' involves a number of obscure points, which, so far as I am aware, are as yet unsolved
enigmas. Up to a certain point, however, any law which is found to hold as to the expression of the emotions
by simple sounds must apply to the more developed mode of expression in song, which may be taken as the
primary type of all music. A great part of the emotional effect of a song depends on the character of the
action by which the sounds are produced. In songs, for instance, which express great vehemence of passion,
the effect often chiefly depends on the forcible utterance of some one or two characteristic passages which
demand great exertion of vocal force; and it will be frequently noticed that a song of this character fails of its
proper effect when sung by a voice of sufficient power and range to give the characteristic passages without
much exertion. This is, no doubt, the secret of the loss of effect so often produced by the transposition of a
song from one key to another. The effect is thus seen to depend not merely on the actual sounds, but also in
part on the nature of the action which produces the sounds. Indeed it is obvious that whenever we feel the
`expression' of a song to be due to its quickness or slowness of movement to smoothness of flow, loudness
of utterance, and so onwe are, in fact, interpreting the muscular actions which produce sound, in the same
way in which we interpret muscular action generally. But this leaves unexplained the more subtle and more
specific effect which we call the MUSICAL expression of the song the delight given by its melody, or
even by the separate sounds which make up the melody. This is an effect indefinable in language one
which, so far as I am aware, no one has been able to analyse, and which the ingenious speculation of Mr.
Herbert Spencer as to the origin of music leaves quite unexplained. For it is certain that the MELODIC effect
of a series of sounds does not depend in the least on their loudness or softness, or on their ABSOLUTE pitch.
A tune is always the same tune, whether it is sung loudly or softly, by a child or a man; whether it is played
on a flute or on a trombone. The purely musical effect of any sound depends on its place in what is
technically called a `scale;' the same sound producing absolutely different effects on the ear, according as it is
heard in connection with one or another series of sounds.
[4] Mr. Tylor (`Primitive Culture,' 1871, vol. i. p. 166), in his discussion on this subject, alludes to the
whining of the dog.
[5] `Naturgeschichte der Saugethiere von Paraguay,' 1830, s. 46.
[6] Quoted by Gratiolet, `De la Physionomie,' 1865, p. 115.
"It is on this RELATIVE association of the sounds that all the essentially characteristic effects which are
summed up in the phrase `musical expression,' depend. But why certain associations of sounds have
suchandsuch effects, is a problem which yet remains to be solved. These effects must indeed, in some way
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 34
Page No 37
or other, be connected with the wellknown arithmetical relations between the rates of vibration of the
sounds which form a musical scale. And it is possiblebut this is merely a suggestionthat the greater or
less mechanical facility with which the vibrating apparatus of the human larynx passes from one state of
vibration to another, may have been a primary cause of the greater or less pleasure produced by various
sequences of sounds."
But leaving aside these complex questions and confining ourselves to the simpler sounds, we can, at least, see
some reasons for the association of certain kinds of sounds with certain states of mind. A scream, for
instance, uttered by a young animal, or by one of the members of a community, as a call for assistance, will
naturally be loud, prolonged, and high, so as to penetrate to a distance. For Helmholtz has shown[7] that,
owing to the shape of the internal cavity of the human ear and its consequent power of resonance, high notes
produce a particularly strong impression. When male animals utter sounds in order to please the females, they
would naturally employ those which are sweet to the ears of the species; and it appears that the same sounds
are often pleasing to widely different animals, owing to the similarity of their nervous systems, as we
ourselves perceive in the singing of birds and even in the chirping of certain treefrogs giving us pleasure.
On the other hand, sounds produced in order to strike terror into an enemy, would naturally be harsh or
displeasing.
Whether the principle of antithesis has come into play with sounds, as might perhaps have been expected, is
doubtful. The interrupted, laughing or tittering sounds made by man and by various kinds of monkeys when
pleased, are as different as possible from the prolonged screams of these animals when distressed. The deep
grunt of satisfaction uttered by a pig, when pleased with its food, is widely different from its harsh scream of
pain or terror. But with the dog, as lately remarked, the bark of anger and that of joy are sounds which by no
means stand in opposition to each other; and so it is in some other cases.
There is another obscure point, namely, whether the sounds which are produced under various states of the
mind determine the shape of the mouth, or whether its shape is not determined by independent causes, and
the sound thus modified. When young infants cry they open their mouths widely, and this, no doubt, is
necessary for pouring forth a full volume of sound; but the mouth then assumes, from a quite distinct cause,
an almost quadrangular shape, depending, as will hereafter be explained, on the firm closing of the eyelids,
and consequent drawing up of the upper lip. How far this square shape of the mouth modifies the wailing or
crying sound, I am not prepared to say; but we know from the researches of Helmholtz and others that the
form of the cavity of the mouth and lips determines the nature and pitch of the vowel sounds which are
produced.
[7] `Theorie Physiologique de la Musique,' Paris, 1868, P. 146. Helmholtz has also fully discussed in this
profound work the relation of the form of the cavity of the mouth to the production of vowelsounds.
It will also be shown in a future chapter that, under the feeling of contempt or disgust, there is a tendency,
from intelligible causes, to blow out of the mouth or nostrils, and this produces sounds like pooh or pish.
When any one is startled or suddenly astonished, there is an instantaneous tendency, likewise from an
intelligible cause, namely, to be ready for prolonged exertion, to open the mouth widely, so as to draw a deep
and rapid inspiration. When the next full expiration follows, the mouth is slightly closed, and the lips, from
causes hereafter to be discussed, are somewhat protruded; and this form of the mouth, if the voice be at all
exerted, produces, according to Helmholtz, the sound of the vowel _O_. Certainly a deep sound of a
prolonged _Oh!_ may be heard from a whole crowd of people immediately after witnessing any astonishing
spectacle. If, together with surprise, pain be felt, there is a tendency to contract all the muscles of the body,
including those of the face, and the lips will then be drawn back; and this will perhaps account for the sound
becoming higher and assuming the character of _Ah!_ or _Ach!_ As fear causes all the muscles of the body
to tremble, the voice naturally becomes tremulous, and at the same time husky from the dryness of the mouth,
owing to the salivary glands failing to act. Why the laughter of man and the tittering of monkeys should be a
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 35
Page No 38
rapidly reiterated sound, cannot be explained. During the utterance of these sounds, the mouth is transversely
elongated by the corners being drawn backwards and upwards; and of this fact an explanation will be
attempted in a future chapter. But the whole subject of the differences of the sounds produced under different
states of the mind is so obscure, that I have succeeded in throwing hardly any light on it; and the remarks
which I have made, have but little significance.
All the sounds hitherto noticed depend on the respiratory organs; but sounds produced by wholly different
means are likewise expressive. Rabbits stamp loudly on the ground as a signal to their comrades; and if a man
knows how to do so properly, he may on a quiet evening hear the rabbits answering him all around. These
animals, as well as some others, also stamp on the ground when made angry. Porcupines rattle their quills and
vibrate their tails when angered; and one behaved in this manner when a live snake was placed in its
compartment. The tail of the quills on the tail are very different from those on the body: they are short,
hollow, thin like a goosequill, with their ends transversely truncated, so that they are open; they are
supported on long, thin, elastic footstalks. Now, when the tail is rapidly shaken, these hollow quills strike
against each other and produce, as I heard in the presence of Mr. Bartlett, a peculiar continuous sound. We
can, I think, understand why porcupines have been provided, through the modification of their protective
spines, with this special soundproducing instrument. They are nocturnal animals, and if they scented or
heard a prowling beast of prey, it would be a great advantage to them in the dark to give warning to their
enemy what they were, and that they were furnished with dangerous spines. They would thus escape being
attacked. They are, as I may add, so fully conscious of the power of their weapons, that when enraged they
will charge backwards with their spines erected, yet still inclined backwards.
Many birds during their courtship produce diversified sounds by means of specially adapted feathers. Storks,
when excited, make a loud clattering noise with their beaks. Some snakes produce a grating or rattling noise.
Many insects stridulate by rubbing together specially modified parts of their hard integuments. This
stridulation generally serves as a sexual charm or call; but it is likewise used to express different emotions.[8]
Every one who has attended to bees knows that their humming changes when they are angry; and this serves
as a warning that there is danger of being stung. I have made these few remarks because some writers have
laid so much stress on the vocal and respiratory organs as having been specially adapted for expression, that it
was advisable to show that sounds otherwise produced serve equally well for the same purpose.
_Erection of the dermal appendages_.Hardly any expressive movement is so general as the involuntary
erection of the hairs, feathers and other dermal appendages; for it is common throughout three of the great
vertebrate classes. These appendages are erected under the excitement of anger or terror; more especially
when these emotions are combined, or quickly succeed each other. The action serves to make the animal
appear larger and more frightful to its enemies or rivals, and is generally accompanied by various voluntary
movements adapted for the same purpose, and by the utterance of savage sounds. Mr. Bartlett, who has had
such wide experience with animals of all kinds, does not doubt that this is the case; but it is a different
question whether the power of erection was primarily acquired for this special purpose.
[8] I have given some details on this subject in my `Descent of Man,' vol. i. pp. 352, 384.
I will first give a considerable body of facts showing how general this action is with mammals, birds and
reptiles; retaining what I have to say in regard to man for a future chapter. Mr. Sutton, the intelligent keeper
in the Zoological Gardens, carefully observed for me the Chimpanzee and Orang; and he states that when
they are suddenly frightened, as by a thunderstorm, or when they are made angry, as by being teased, their
hair becomes erect. I saw a chimpanzee who was alarmed at the sight of a black coalheaver, and the hair rose
all over his body; he made little starts forward as if to attack the man, without any real intention of doing so,
but with the hope, as the keeper remarked, of frightening him. The Gorilla, when enraged, is described by Mr.
Ford[9] as having his crest of hair "erect and projecting forward, his nostrils dilated, and his under lip thrown
down; at the same time uttering his characteristic yell, designed, it would seem, to terrify his antagonists." I
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 36
Page No 39
saw the hair on the Anubis baboon, when angered bristling along the back, from the neck to the loins, but not
on the rump or other parts of the body. I took a stuffed snake into the monkeyhouse, and the hair on several
of the species instantly became erect; especially on their tails, as I particularly noticed with the
_Cereopithecus nictitans_. Brehm states[10] that the _Midas aedipus_ (belonging to the American division)
when excited erects its mane, in order, as he adds, to make itself as frightful as possible.
[9] As quoted in Huxley's `Evidence as to Man's Place in Nature,' 1863, p. 52.
With the Carnivora the erection of the hair seems to be almost universal, often accompanied by threatening
movements, the uncovering of the teeth and the utterance of savage growls. In the Herpestes, I have seen the
hair on end over nearly the whole body, including the tail; and the dorsal crest is erected in a conspicuous
manner by the Hyaena and Proteles. The enraged lion erects his mane. The bristling of the hair along the neck
and back of the dog, and over the whole body of the cat, especially on the tail, is familiar to every one. With
the cat it apparently occurs only under fear; with the dog, under anger and fear; but not, as far as I have
observed, under abject fear, as when a dog is going to be flogged by a severe gamekeeper. If, however, the
dog shows fight, as sometimes happens, up goes his hair. I have often noticed that the hair of a dog is
particularly liable to rise, if he is half angry and half afraid, as on beholding some object only indistinctly
seen in the dusk.
I have been assured by a veterinary surgeon that he has often seen the hair erected on horses and cattle, on
which he had operated and was again going to operate. When I showed a stuffed snake to a Peccary, the hair
rose in a wonderful manner along its back; and so it does with the boar when enraged. An Elk which gored a
man to death in the United States, is described as first brandishing his antlers, squealing with rage and
stamping on the ground; "at length his hair was seen to rise and stand on end," and then he plunged forward
to the attack.[11] The hair likewise becomes erect on goats, and, as I hear from Mr. Blyth, on some Indian
antelopes. I have seen it erected on the hairy Anteater; and on the Agouti, one of the Rodents. A female
Bat,[12] which reared her young under confinement, when any one looked into the cage "erected the fur on
her back, and bit viciously at intruding fingers."
[10] Illust. Thierleben, 1864, B. i. s. 130.
Birds belonging to all the chief Orders ruffle their feathers when angry or frightened. Every one must have
seen two cocks, even quite young birds, preparing to fight with erected neckhackles; nor can these feathers
when erected serve as a means of defence, for cockfighters have found by experience that it is advantageous
to trim them. The male Ruff (_Machetes pugnax_) likewise erects its collar of feathers when fighting. When a
dog approaches a common hen with her chickens, she spreads out her wings, raises her tail, ruffles all her
feathers, and looking as ferocious as possible, dashes at the intruder. The tail is not always held in exactly the
same position; it is sometimes so much erected, that the central feathers, as in the accompanying drawing,
almost touch the back. Swans, when angered, likewise raise their wings and tail, and erect their feathers.
They open their beaks, and make by paddling little rapid starts forwards, against any one who approaches the
water's edge too closely. Tropic birds[13] when disturbed on their nests are said not to fly away, but "merely
to stick out their feathers and scream." The Barnowl, when approached "instantly swells out its plumage,
extends its wings and tail, hisses and clacks its mandibles with force and rapidity."[14] So do other kinds of
owls. Hawks, as I am
[11] The Hon. J. Caton, Ottawa Acad. of Nat. Sciences, May, 1868, pp. 36, 40. For the _Capra, AEgagrus_,
`Land and Water,' 1867, p. 37.
[12] `Land and Water,' July 20, 1867, p. 659.
[13] _Phaeton rubricauda_: `Ibis,' vol. iii. 1861, p. 180.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 37
Page No 40
{illust. caption = FIG. 12Hen driving away a dog from her chickens. Drawn from life by Mr. Wood.
informed by Mr. Jenner Weir, likewise ruffle their feathers, and spread out their wings and tail under similar
circumstances. Some kinds of parrots erect their feathers; and I have seen this action in the Cassowary, when
angered at the sight of an Anteater. Young cuckoos in the nest, raise their feathers, open their mouths
widely, and make themselves as frightful as possible. [14] On the _Strix flammea_, Audubon, `Ornithological
Biography,' 1864, vol. ii. p. 407. I have observed other cases in the Zoological Gardens.Small birds, also, as I
hear from Mr. Weir, such as various finches, buntings and warblers, when angry, {illust. caption = FIG.
13.Swan driving away an intruder. Drawn from life by Mr. Wood.}
ruffle all their feathers, or only those round the neck; or they spread out their wings and tailfeathers. With
their plumage in this state, they rush at each other with open beaks and threatening gestures. Mr. Weir
concludes from his large experience that the erection of the feathers is caused much more by anger than by
fear. He gives as an instance a hybrid goldfinch of a most irascible disposition, which when approached too
closely by a servant, instantly assumes the appearance of a ball of ruffled feathers. He believes that birds
when frightened, as a general rule, closely adpress all their feathers, and their consequently diminished size is
often astonishing. As soon as they recover from their fear or surprise, the first thing which they do is to shake
out their feathers. The best instances of this adpression of the feathers and apparent shrinking of the body
from fear, which Mr. Weir has noticed, has been in the quail and grassparrakeet.[15] The habit is intelligible
in these birds from their being accustomed, when in danger, either to squat on the ground or to sit motionless
on a branch, so as to escape detection. Though, with birds, anger may be the chief and commonest cause of
the erection of the feathers, it is probable that young cuckoos when looked at in the nest, and a hen with her
chickens when approached by a dog, feel at least some terror. Mr. Tegetmeier informs me that with
gamecocks, the erection of the feathers on the head has long been recognized in the cockpit as a sign of
cowardice.
The males of some lizards, when fighting together during their courtship, expand their throat pouches or
frills, and erect their dorsal crests.[16] But Dr. Gunther does not believe that they can erect their separate
spines or scales.
We thus see how generally throughout the two higher vertebrate classes, and with some reptiles, the dermal
appendages are erected under the influence of anger and fear. The movement is effected, as we know from
Kolliker's interesting discovery, by the contraction of minute, unstriped, involuntary muscles,[17] often called
_arrectores pili_, which are attached to the capsules of the separate hairs, feathers, By the contraction of these
muscles the hairs can be instantly erected, as we see in a dog, being at the same time drawn a little out of their
sockets; they are afterwards quickly depressed. The vast number of these minute muscles over the whole
body of a hairy quadruped is astonishing. The erection of the hair is, however, aided in some cases, as with
that on the head of a man, by the striped and voluntary muscles of the underlying _panniculus carnosus_. It is
by the action of these latter muscles, that the hedgehog erects its spines. It appears, also, from the researches
of Leydig[18] and others, that striped fibres extend from the panniculus to some of the larger hairs, such as
the vibrissae of certain quadrupeds. The _arrectores pili_ contract not only under the above emotions, but
from the application of cold to the surface. I remember that my mules and dogs, brought from a lower and
warmer country, after spending a night on the bleak Cordillera, had the hair all over their bodies as erect as
under the greatest terror. We see the same action in our own _gooseskin_ during the chill before a feverfit.
Mr. Lister has also found,[19] that tickling a neighbouring part of the skin causes the erection and protrusion
of the hairs.
[15] _Melopsittacus undulatus_. See an account of its habits by Gould, `Handbook of Birds of Australia,'
1865, vol. ii. p. 82.
[16] See, for instance, the account which I have given (`Descent of Man,' vol. ii. p. 32) of an Anolis and
Draco.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 38
Page No 41
[17] These muscles are described in his wellknown works. I am greatly indebted to this distinguished
observer for having given me in a letter information on this same subject.
From these facts it is manifest that the erection of the dermal appendages is a reflex action, independent of
the will; and this action must be looked at, when, occurring under the influence of anger or fear, not as a
power acquired for the sake of some advantage, but as an incidental result, at least to a large extent, of the
sensorium being affected. The result, in as far as it is incidental, may be compared with the profuse sweating
from an agony of pain or terror. Nevertheless, it is remarkable how slight an excitement often suffices to
cause the hair to become erect; as when two dogs pretend to fight together in play. We have, also, seen in a
large number of animals, belonging to widely distinct classes, that the erection of the hair or feathers is
almost always accompanied by various voluntary movements by threatening gestures, opening the mouth,
uncovering the teeth, spreading out of the wings and tail by birds, and by the utterance of harsh sounds; and
the purpose of these voluntary movements is unmistakable. Therefore it seems hardly credible that the
coordinated erection of the dermal appendages, by which the animal is made to appear larger and more
terrible to its enemies or rivals, should be altogether an incidental and purposeless result of the disturbance of
the sensorium. This seems almost as incredible as that the erection by the hedgehog of its spines, or of the
quills by the porcupine, or of the ornamental plumes by many birds during their courtship. should all be
purposeless actions.
[18] `Lehrbuch der Histologie des Menschen,' 1857, s. 82. I owe to Prof. W. Turner's kindness an extract
from this work.
[19] `Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science,' 1853, vol. i. p. 262.
We here encounter a great difficulty. How can the contraction of the unstriped and involuntary _arrectores
pili_ have been coordinated with that of various voluntary muscles for the same special purpose? If we
could believe that the arrectores primordially had been voluntary muscles, and had since lost their stripes and
become involuntary, the case would be comparatively simple. I am not, however, aware that there is any
evidence in favour of this view; although the reversed transition would not have presented any great
difficulty, as the voluntary muscles are in an unstriped condition in the embryos of the higher animals, and in
the larvae of some crustaceans. Moreover in the deeper layers of the skin of adult birds, the muscular network
is, according to Leydig,[20] in a transitional condition; the fibres exhibiting only indications of transverse
striation.
Another explanation seems possible. We may admit that originally the _arrectores pili_ were slightly acted on
in a direct manner, under the influence of rage and terror, by the disturbance of the nervous system; as is
undoubtedly the case with our socalled _gooseskin_ before a feverfit. Animals have been repeatedly
excited by rage and terror during many generations; and consequently the direct effects of the disturbed
nervous system on the dermal appendages will almost certainly have been increased through habit and
through the tendency of nerveforce to pass readily along accustomed channels. We shall find this view of
the force of habit strikingly confirmed in a future chapter, where it will be shown that the hair of the insane is
affected in an extraordinary manner, owing to their repeated accesses of fury and terror. As soon as with
animals the power of erection had thus been strengthened or increased, they must often have seen the hairs or
feathers erected in rival and enraged males, and the bulk of their bodies thus increased. In this case it appears
possible that they might have wished to make themselves appear larger and more terrible to their enemies, by
voluntarily assuming a threatening attitude and uttering harsh cries; such attitudes and utterances after a time
becoming through habit instinctive. In this manner actions performed by the contraction of voluntary muscles
might have been combined for the same special purpose with those effected by involuntary muscles. It is
even possible that animals, when excited and dimly conscious of some change in the state of their hair, might
act on it by repeated exertions of their attention and will; for we have reason to believe that the will is able to
influence in an obscure manner the action of some unstriped or involuntary muscles, as in the period of the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 39
Page No 42
peristaltic movements of the intestines, and in the contraction of the bladder. Nor must we overlook the part
which variation and natural selection may have played; for the males which succeeded in making themselves
appear the most terrible to their rivals, or to their other enemies, if not of overwhelming power, will on an
average have left more offspring to inherit their characteristic qualities, whatever these may be and however
first acquired, than have other males.
[20] `Lehrbuch der Histologie,' 1857, s. 82.
_The inflation of the body, and other means of exciting fear in an enemy_.Certain Amphibians and
Reptiles, which either have no spines to erect, or no muscles by which they can be erected, enlarge
themselves when alarmed or angry by inhaling air. This is well known to be the case with toads and frogs.
The latter animal is made, in AEsop's fable of the `Ox and the Frog,' to blow itself up from vanity and envy
until it burst. This action must have been observed during the most ancient times, as, according to Mr.
Hensleigh Wedgwood,[21] the word _toad_ expresses in all the languages of Europe the habit of swelling. It
has been observed with some of the exotic species in the Zoological Gardens; and Dr. Gunther believes that it
is general throughout the group. Judging from analogy, the primary purpose probably was to make the body
appear as large and frightful as possible to an enemy; but another, and perhaps more important secondary
advantage is thus gained. When frogs are seized by snakes, which are their chief enemies, they enlarge
themselves wonderfully; so that if the snake be of small size, as Dr. Gunther informs me, it cannot swallow
the frog, which thus escapes being devoured.
[21] `Dictionary of English Etymology,' p. 403.
Chameleons and some other lizards inflate themselves when angry. Thus a species inhabiting Oregon, the
_Tapaya Douglasii_, is slow in its movements and does not bite, but has a ferocious aspect; "when irritated it
springs in a most threatening manner at anything pointed at it, at the same time opening its mouth wide and
hissing audibly, after which it inflates its body, and shows other marks of anger."[22]
Several kinds of snakes likewise inflate themselves when irritated. The puffadder (_Clotho arietans_) is
remarkable in this respect; but I believe, after carefully watching these animals, that they do not act thus for
the sake of increasing their apparent bulk, but simply for inhaling a large supply of air, so as to produce their
surprisingly loud, harsh, and prolonged hissing sound. The Cobrasdecapello, when irritated, enlarge
themselves a little, and hiss moderately; but, at the same time they lift their heads aloft, and dilate by means
of their elongated anterior ribs, the skin on each side of the neck into a large flat disk,the socalled hood.
With their widely opened mouths, they then assume a terrific aspect. The benefit thus derived ought to be
considerable, in order to compensate for the somewhat lessened rapidity (though this is still great) with
which, when dilated, they can strike at their enemies or prey; on the same principle that a broad, thin piece of
wood cannot be moved through the air so quickly as a small round stick. An innocuous snake, the
_Trovidonotus macrophthalmus_, an inhabitant of India, likewise dilates its neck when irritated; and
consequently is often mistaken for its compatriot, the deadly Cobra.[23] This resemblance perhaps serves as
some protection to the Tropidonotus.
[21] See the account of the habits of this animal by Dr, Cooper, as quoted in `Nature,' April 27, 1871, p. 512.
[22] Dr. Gunther, `Reptiles of British India,' p. 262.
Another innocuous species, the Dasypeltis of South Africa, blows itself out, distends its neck, hisses and darts
at an intruder.[24] Many other snakes hiss under similar circumstances. They also rapidly vibrate their
protruded tongues; and this may aid in increasing their terrific appearance.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 40
Page No 43
Snakes possess other means of producing sounds besides hissing. Many years ago I observed in South
America that a venomous Trigonocephalus, when disturbed, rapidly vibrated the end of its tail, which striking
against the dry grass and twigs produced a rattling noise that could be distinctly heard at the distance of six
feet.[25] The deadly and fierce _Echis carinata_ of India produces "a curious prolonged, almost hissing sound
in a very different manner, namely by rubbing "the sides of the folds of its body against each other," whilst
the head remains in almost the same position. The scales on the sides, and not on other parts of the body, are
strongly keeled, with the keels toothed like a saw; and as the coiledup animal rubs its sides together, these
grate against each other.[26] Lastly, we have the wellknown case of the Rattlesnake. He who has merely
shaken the rattle of a dead snake, can form no just idea of the sound produced by the living animal. Professor
Shaler states that it is indistinguishable from that made by the male of a large Cicada (an Homopterous
insect), which inhabits the same district.[27] In the Zoological Gardens, when the rattlesnakes and
puffadders were greatly excited at the same time, I was much struck at the similarity of the sound produced
by them; and although that made by the rattlesnake is louder and shriller than the hissing of the puffadder,
yet when standing at some yards distance I could scarcely distinguish the two. For whatever purpose the
sound is produced by the one species, I can hardly doubt that it serves for the same purpose in the other
species; and I conclude from the threatening gestures made at the same time by many snakes, that their
hissing,the rattling of the rattlesnake and of the tail of the Trigonocephalus,the grating of the scales of
the Echis,and the dilatation of the hood of the Cobra, all subserve the same end, namely, to make them
appear terrible to their enemies.[28]
[24] Mr. J. Mansel Weale, `Nature,' April 27, 1871, p. 508.
[25] `Journal of Researches during the Voyage of the "Beagle," ' 1845, p. 96. I have compared the rattling
thus produced with that of the Rattlesnake.
[26] See the account by Dr. Anderson, Proc. Zool. Soc. 1871, p. 196.
[27] The `American Naturalist,' Jan. 1872, p. 32. I regret that I cannot follow Prof. Shaler in believing that the
rattle has been developed, by the aid of natural selection, for the sake of producing sounds which deceive and
attract birds, so that they may serve as prey to the snake.
It seems at first a probable conclusion that venomous snakes, such as the foregoing, from being already so
well defended by their poisonfangs, would never be attacked by any enemy; and consequently would have
{note [27] continued} I do not, however, wish to doubt that the sounds may occasionally subserve this end.
But the conclusion at which I have arrived, viz. that the rattling serves as a warning to wouldbe devourers,
appears to me much more probable, as it connects together various classes of facts. If this snake had acquired
its rattle and the habit of rattling, for the sake of attracting prey, it does not seem probable that it would have
invariably used its instrument when angered or disturbed. Prof. Shaler takes nearly the same view as I do of
the manner of development of the rattle; and I have always held this opinion since observing the
Trigonocephalus in South America.
[28] From the accounts lately collected, and given in the `Journal of the Linnean Society,' by Airs. Barber, on
the habits of the snakes of South Africa; and from the accounts published by several writers, for instance by
Lawson, of the rattlesnake in North America,it does not seem improbable that the terrific appearance of
snakes and the sounds produced by them, may likewise serve in procuring prey, by paralysing, or as it is
sometimes called fascinating, the smaller animals. no need to excite additional terror. But this is far from
being the case, for they are largely preyed on in all quarters of the world by many animals. It is well known
that pigs are employed in the United States to clear districts infested with rattlesnakes, which they do most
effectually.[29] In England the hedgehog attacks and devours the viper. In India, as I hear from Dr. Jerdon,
several kinds of hawks, and at least one mammal, the Herpestes, kill cobras and other venomous species;[30]
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 41
Page No 44
and so it is in South Africa. Therefore it is by no means improbable that any sounds or signs by which the
venomous species could instantly make themselves recognized as dangerous, would be of more service to
them than to the innocuous species which would not be able, if attacked, to inflict any real injury.
Having said thus much about snakes, I am tempted to add a few remarks on the means by which the rattle of
the rattlesnake was probably developed. Various animals, including some lizards, either curl or vibrate their
tails when excited. This is the case with many kinds of snakes.[31] In the Zoological Gardens, an innocuous
species, the _Coronella Sayi_, vibrates its tail so rapidly that it becomes almost invisible. The
Trigonocephalus, before alluded to, has the same habit; and the extremity of its tail is a little enlarged, or ends
in a bead. In the Lachesis, which is so closely allied to the rattlesnake that it was placed by Linnaeus in the
same genus, the tail ends in a single, large, lancetshaped point or scale. With some snakes the skin, as
Professor Shaler remarks, "is more imperfectly detached from the region about the tail than at other parts of
the body." Now if we suppose that the end of the tail of some ancient American species was enlarged, and
was covered by a single large scale, this could hardly have been cast off at the successive moults. In this case
it would have been permanently retained, and at each period of growth, as the snake grew larger, a new scale,
larger than the last, would have been formed above it, and would likewise have been retained. The foundation
for the development of a rattle would thus have been laid; and it would have been habitually used, if the
species, like so many others, vibrated its tail whenever it was irritated. That the rattle has since been specially
developed to serve as an efficient soundproducing instrument, there can hardly be a doubt; for even the
vertebrae included within the extremity of the tail have been altered in shape and cohere. But there is no
greater improbability in various structures, such as the rattle of the rattlesnake, the lateral scales of the
Echis,the neck with the included ribs of the Cobra,and the whole body of the puffadder,having been
modified for the sake of warning and frightening away their enemies, than in a bird, namely, the wonderful
Secretaryhawk (_Gypogeranus_) having had its whole frame modified for the sake of killing snakes with
impunity. It is highly probable, judging from what we have before seen, that this bird would ruffle its feathers
whenever it attacked a snake; and it is certain that the Herpestes, when it eagerly rushes to attack a snake,
erects the hair all over its body, and especially that on its tail.[32] We have also seen that some porcupines,
when angered or alarmed at the sight of a snake, rapidly vibrate their tails, thus producing a peculiar sound by
the striking together of the hollow quills. So that here both the attackers and the attacked endeavour to make
themselves as dreadful as possible to each other; and both possess for this purpose specialised means, which,
oddly enough, are nearly the same in some of these cases. Finally we can see that if, on the one hand, those
individual snakes, which were best able to frighten away their enemies, escaped best from being devoured;
and if, on the other hand, those individuals of the attacking enemy survived in larger numbers which were the
best fitted for the dangerous task of killing and devouring venomous snakes; then in the one case as in the
other, beneficial variations, supposing the characters in question to vary, would commonly have been
preserved through the survival of the fittest.
[29] See the account by Dr. R. Brown, in Proc. Zool. Soc. 1871, p. 39. He says that as soon as a pig sees a
snake it rushes upon it; and a snake makes off immediately on the appearance of a pig.
[30] Dr. Gunther remarks (`Reptiles of British India,' p. 340) on the destruction of cobras by the ichneumon
or herpestes, and whilst the cobras are young by the junglefowl. It is well known that the peacock also
eagerly kills snakes.
[31] Prof. Cope enumerates a number of kinds in his `Method of Creation of Organic Types,' read before the
American Phil. Soc., December 15th, 1871, p. 20. Prof. Cope takes the same view as I do of the use of the
gestures and sounds made by snakes. I briefly alluded to this subject in the last edition of my `Origin of
Species.' Since the passages in the text above have been printed, I have been pleased to find that Mr.
Henderson (`The American Naturalist,' May, 1872, p. 260) also takes a similar view of the use of the rattle,
namely "in preventing an attack from being made."
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 42
Page No 45
_The Drawing back and pressure of the Ears to the Head_.The ears through their movements are highly
expressive in many animals; but in some, such as man, the higher apes, and many ruminants, they fail in this
respect. A slight difference in position serves to express in the plainest manner a different state of mind, as
we may daily see in the dog; but we are here concerned only with the ears being drawn closely backwards
and pressed to the head. A savage frame of mind is thus shown, but only in the case of those animals which
fight with their teeth; and the care which they take to prevent their ears being seized by their antagonists,
accounts for this position. Consequently, through habit and association, whenever they feel slightly savage, or
pretend in their play to be savage, their ears are drawn back. That this is the true explanation may be inferred
from the relation which exists in very many animals between their manner of fighting and the retraction of
their ears.
[32] Mr. des Voeux, in Proc. Zool. Soc. 1871, p. 3.
All the Carnivora fight with their canine teeth, and all, as far as I have observed, draw their ears back when
feeling savage. This may be continually seen with dogs when fighting in earnest, and with puppies fighting in
play. The movement is different from the falling down and slight drawing back of the ears, when a dog feels
pleased and is caressed by his master. The retraction of the ears may likewise be seen in kittens fighting
together in their play, and in fullgrown cats when really savage, as before illustrated in fig. 9 (p. 58).
Although their ears are thus to a large extent protected, yet they often get much torn in old male cats during
their mutual battles. The same movement is very striking in tigers, leopards, whilst growling over their food
in menageries. The lynx has remarkably long ears; and their retraction, when one of these animals is
approached in its cage, is very conspicuous, and is eminently expressive of its savage disposition. Even one
of the Eared Seals, the _Otariapusilla_, which has very small ears, draws them backwards, when it makes a
savage rush at the legs of its keeper.
When horses fight together they use their incisors for biting, and their forelegs for striking, much more than
they do their hindlegs for kicking backwards. This has been observed when stallions have broken loose and
have fought together, and may likewise be inferred from the kind of wounds which they inflict on each other.
Every one recognizes the vicious appearance which the drawing back of the ears gives to a horse. This
movement is very different from that of listening to a sound behind. If an illtempered horse in a stall is
inclined to kick backwards, his ears are retracted from habit, though he has no intention or power to bite. But
when a horse throws up both hindlegs in play, as when entering an open field, or when just touched by the
whip, he does not generally depress his ears, for he does not then feel vicious. Guanacoes fight savagely with
their teeth; and they must do so frequently, for I found the hides of several which I shot in Patagonia deeply
scored. So do camels; and both these animals, when savage, draw their ears closely backwards. Guanacoes, as
I have noticed, when not intending to bite, but merely to spit their offensive saliva from a distance at an
intruder, retract their ears. Even the hippopotamus, when threatening with its widelyopen enormous mouth a
comrade, draws back its small ears, just like a horse.
Now what a contrast is presented between the foregoing animals and cattle, sheep, or goats, which never use
their teeth in fighting, and never draw back their ears when enraged! Although sheep and goats appear such
placid animals, the males often join in furious contests. As deer form a closely related family, and as I did not
know that they ever fought with their teeth, I was much surprised at the account given by Major Ross King of
the Moosedeer in Canada. He says, when "two males chance to meet, laying back their ears and gnashing
their teeth together, they rush at each other with appalling fury."[33] But Mr. Bartlett informs me that some
species of deer fight savagely with their teeth, so that the drawing back of the ears by the moose accords with
our rule. Several kinds of kangaroos, kept in the Zoological Gardens, fight by scratching with their forefeet
and by kicking with their hindlegs; but they never bite each other, and the keepers have never seen them
draw back their ears when angered. Rabbits fight chiefly by kicking and scratching, but they likewise bite
each other; and I have known one to bite off half the tail of its antagonist. At the commencement of their
battles they lay back their ears, but afterwards, as they bound over and kick each other, they keep their ears
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS. 43
Page No 46
erect, or move them much about.
[33] `The Sportsman and Naturalist in Canada,' 1866, p. 53. p. 53.{sic}
Mr. Bartlett watched a wild boar quarrelling rather savagely with his sow; and both had their mouths open
and their ears drawn backwards. But this does not appear to be a common action with domestic pigs when
quarrelling. Boars fight together by striking upwards with their tusks; and Mr. Bartlett doubts whether they
then draw back their ears. Elephants, which in like manner fight with their tusks, do not retract their ears, but,
on the contrary, erect them when rushing at each other or at an enemy.
The rhinoceroses in the Zoological Gardens fight with their nasal horns, and have never been seen to attempt
biting each other except in play; and the keepers are convinced that they do not draw back their ears, like
horses and dogs, when feeling savage. The following statement, therefore, by Sir S. Baker[34] is inexplicable,
namely, that a rhinoceros, which he shot in North Africa, "had no ears; they had been bitten off close to the
head by another of the same species while fighting; and this mutilation is by no means uncommon."
Lastly, with respect to monkeys. Some kinds, which have moveable ears, and which fight with their
teethfor instance the _Cereopithecus ruber_ draw back their ears when irritated just like dogs; and they
then have a very spiteful appearance. Other kinds, as the _Inuus ecaudatus_, apparently do not thus act.
Again, other kindsand this is a great anomaly in comparison with most other animalsretract their ears,
show their teeth, and jabber, when they are pleased by being caressed. I observed this in two or three species
of Macacus, and in the _Cynopithecus niger_. This expression, owing to our familiarity with dogs, would
never be recognized as one of joy or pleasure by those unacquainted with monkeys.
[34] `The Nile Tributaries of Abyssinia,' 1867, p. 443.
_Erection of the Ears_.This movement requires hardly any notice. All animals which have the power of
freely moving their ears, when they are startled, or when they closely observe any object, direct their ears to
the point towards which they are looking, in order to hear any sound from this quarter. At the same time they
generally raise their heads, as all their organs of sense are there situated, and some of the smaller animals rise
on their hindlegs. Even those kinds which squat on the ground or instantly flee away to avoid danger,
generally act momentarily in this manner, in order to ascertain the source and nature of the danger. The head
being raised, with erected ears and eyes directed forwards, gives an unmistakable expression of close
attention to any animal.
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS.
The Dog, various expressive movements ofCatsHorsesRuminantsMonkeys, their expression of joy
and affectionOf painAngerAstonishment and Terror.
_The Dog_.I have already described (figs. 5 and 1) the appearance of a dog approaching another dog with
hostile intentions, namely, with erected ears, eyes intently directed forwards, hair on the neck and back
bristling, gait remarkably stiff, with the tail upright and rigid. So familiar is this appearance to us, that an
angry man is sometimes said "to have his back up." Of the above points, the stiff gait and upright tail alone
require further discussion. Sir C. Bell remarks[1] that, when a tiger or wolf is struck by its keeper and is
suddenly roused to ferocity, every muscle is in tension, and the limbs are in an attitude of strained exertion,
prepared to spring. This tension of the muscles and consequent stiff gait may be accounted for on the
principle of associated habit, for anger has continually led to fierce struggles, and consequently to all the
muscles of the body having been violently exerted. There is also reason to suspect that the muscular system
requires some short preparation, or some degree of innervation, before being brought into strong action. My
own sensations lead me to this inference; but I cannot discover that it is a conclusion admitted by
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 44
Page No 47
physiologists. Sir J. Paget, however, informs me that when muscles are suddenly contracted with the greatest
force, without any preparation, they are liable to be ruptured, as when a man slips unexpectedly; but that this
rarely occurs when an action, however violent, is deliberately performed.
[1] `The Anatomy of Expression,' 1844, p. 190.
With respect to the upright position of the tail, it seems to depend (but whether this is really the case I know
not) on the elevator muscles being more powerful than the depressors, so that when all the muscles of the
hinder part of the body are in a state of tension, the tail is raised. A dog in cheerful spirits, and trotting before
his master with high, elastic steps, generally carries his tail aloft, though it is not held nearly so stiffly as
when he is angered. A horse when first turned out into an open field, may be seen to trot with long elastic
strides, the head and tail being held high aloft. Even cows when they frisk about from pleasure, throw up their
tails in a ridiculous fashion. So it is with various animals in the Zoological Gardens. The position of the tail,
however, in certain cases, is determined by special circumstances; thus as soon as a horse breaks into a
gallop, at full speed, he always lowers his tail, so that as little resistance as possible may be offered to the air.
When a dog is on the point of springing on his antagonist, be utters a savage growl; the ears are pressed
closely backwards, and the upper lip (fig. 14) is retracted out of the way of his teeth, especially of his canines.
These movements may be observed with dogs and puppies in their play. But if a dog gets really savage in his
play, his expression immediately changes. This, however, is simply due to the lips and ears being drawn back
with much greater energy. If a dog only snarls at another, the lip is generally retracted on one side alone,
namely towards his enemy.
The movements of a dog whilst exhibiting affection towards his master were described (figs. 6 and 8) in our
second chapter. These consist in the head and whole body being lowered and thrown into flexuous
movements, with the tail extended and wagged from side to side. The ears fall down and are drawn somewhat
backwards, which causes the eyelids to be elongated, and alters the
{illust. caption = FIG. 14.Head of snarling Dog. From life, by Mr. Wood. whole appearance of the face.
The lips hang loosely, and the hair remains smooth. All these movements or gestures are explicable, as I
believe, from their standing in complete antithesis to those naturally assumed by a savage dog under a
directly opposite state of mind. When a man merely speaks to, or just notices, his dog,we see the last vestige
of these movements in a slight wag of the tail, without any other movement of the body, and without even the
ears being lowered. Dogs also exhibit their affection by desiring to rub against their masters, and to be rubbed
or patted by them. Gratiolet explains the above gestures of affection in the following manner: and the reader
can judge whether the explanation appears satisfactory. Speaking of animals in general, including the dog, he
says,[2] "C'est toujours la partie la plus sensible de leurs corps qui recherche les caresses ou les donne.
Lorsque toute la longueur des flancs et du corps est sensible, l'animal serpente et rampe sous les caresses; et
ces ondulations se propageant le long des muscles analogues des segments jusqu'aux extremites de la colonne
vertebrale, la queue se ploie et s'agite." Further on, he adds, that dogs, when feeling affectionate, lower their
ears in order to exclude all sounds, so that their whole attention may be concentrated on the caresses of their
master! Dogs have another and striking way of exhibiting their affection, namely, by licking the hands or
faces of their masters. They sometimes lick other dogs, and then it is always their chops. I have also seen
dogs licking cats with whom they were friends. This habit probably originated in the females carefully licking
their puppiesthe dearest object of their lovefor the sake of cleansing them. They also often give their
puppies, after a short absence, a few cursory licks, apparently from affection. Thus the habit will have
become associated with the emotion of love, however it may afterwards be aroused. It is now so firmly
inherited or innate, That it is transmitted equally to both sexes. A female terrier of mine lately had her
puppies destroyed, and though at all times a very affectionate creature, I was much struck with the manner in
which she then tried to satisfy her instinctive maternal love by expending it on me; and her desire to lick my
hands rose to an insatiable passion. [1] `De la Physionomie,' 1865, pp. 187, 218.The same principle probably
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 45
Page No 48
explains why dogs, when feeling affectionate, like rubbing against their masters and being rubbed or patted
by them, for from the nursing of their puppies, contact with a beloved object has become firmly associated in
their minds with the emotion of love. The feeling of affection of a dog towards his master is combined with a
strong sense of submission, which is akin to fear. Hence dogs not only lower their bodies and crouch a little
as they approach their masters, but sometimes throw themselves on the ground with their bellies upwards.
This is a movement as completely opposite as is possible to any show of resistance. I formerly possessed a
large dog who was not at all afraid to fight with other dogs; but a wolflike shepherddog in the
neighbourhood, though not ferocious and not so powerful as my dog, had a strange influence over him. When
they met on the road, my dog used to run to meet him, with his tail partly tucked in between his legs and hair
not erected; and then be would throw himself on the ground, belly upwards. By this action he seemed to say
more plainly than by words, "Behold, I am your slave." A pleasurable and excited state of mind, associated
with affection, is exhibited by some dogs in a very peculiar manner, namely, by grinning. This was noticed
long ago by Somerville, who says, And with a courtly grin, the fawning bound Salutes thee cow'ring, his
wide op'ning nose Upward he curls, and his large sloeback eyes Melt in soft blandishments, and humble
joy.' _The Chase_, book i.Sir W. Scott's famous Scotch greyhound, Maida, had this habit, and it is common
with terriers. I have also seen it in a Spitz and in a sheepdog. Mr. Riviere, who has particularly attended to
this expression, informs me that it is rarely displayed in a perfect manner, but is quite common in a lesser
degree. The upper lip during the act of grinning is retracted, as in snarling, so that the canines are exposed,
and the ears are drawn backwards; but the general appearance of the animal clearly shows that anger is not
felt. Sir C. Bell[3] remarks "Dogs, in their expression of fondness, have a slight eversion of the lips, and grin
and sniff amidst their gambols, in a way that resembles laughter." Some persons speak of the grin as a smile,
but if it had been really a smile, we should see a similar, though more pronounced, movement of the lips and
ears, when dogs utter their bark of joy; but this is not the case, although a bark of joy often follows a grin. On
the other hand, dogs, when playing with their comrades or masters, almost always pretend to bite each other;
and they then retract, though not energetically, their lips and ears. Hence I suspect that there is a tendency in
some dogs, whenever they feel lively pleasure combined with affection, to act through habit and association
on the same muscles, as in playfully biting each other, or their masters' hands. I have described, in the second
chapter, the gait and appearance of a dog when cheerful, and the marked antithesis presented by the same
animal when dejected and disappointed, with his head, ears, body, tail, and chops drooping, and eyes dull.
Under the expectation of any great pleasure, dogs bound and jump about in an extravagant manner, and bark
for joy. The tendency to bark under this state of mind is inherited, or runs in the breed: greyhounds rarely
bark, whilst the Spitzdog barks so incessantly on starting for a walk with his master that he becomes a
nuisance. [1] `The Anatomy of Expression,' 1844, p. 140.An agony of pain is expressed by dogs in nearly the
same way as by many other animals, namely, by howling writhing, and contortions of the whole body.
Attention is shown by the head being raised, with the ears erected, and eyes intently directed towards the
object or quarter under observation. If it be a sound and the source is not known, the head is often turned
obliquely from side to side in a most significant manner, apparently in order to judge with more exactness
from what point the sound proceeds. But I have seen a dog greatly surprised at a new noise, turning, his head
to one side through habit, though he clearly perceived the source of the noise. Dogs, as formerly remarked,
when their attention is in any way aroused, whilst watching some object, or attending to some sound, often
lift up one paw (fig. 4) and keep it doubled up, as if to make a slow and stealthy approach. A dog under
extreme terror will throw himself down, howl, and void his excretions; but the hair, I believe, does not
become erect unless some anger is felt. I have seen a dog much terrified at a band of musicians who were
playing loudly outside the house, with every muscle of his body trembling, with his heart palpitating so
quickly that the beats could hardly be counted, and panting for breath with widely open mouth, in the same
manner as a terrified man does. Yet this dog had not exerted himself; he had only wandered slowly and
restlessly about the room, and the day was cold. Even a very slight degree of fear is invariably shown by the
tail being tucked in between the legs. This tucking in of the fail is accompanied by the ears being drawn
backwards; but they are not pressed closely to the head,as in snarling, and they are not lowered, as when a
dog is pleased or affectionate. When two young dogs chase each other in play, the one that runs away always
keeps his tail tucked inwards. So it is when a dog, in the highest spirits, careers like a mad creature round and
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 46
Page No 49
round his master in circles, or in figures of eight. He then acts as if another dog were chasing him. This
curious kind of play, which must be familiar to every one who has attended to dogs, is particularly apt to be
excited, after the animal has been a little startled or frightened, as by his master suddenly jumping out on him
in the dusk. In this case, as well as when two young dogs are chasing each other in play, it appears as if the
one that runs away was afraid of the other catching him by the tail; but as far as I can find out, dogs very
rarely catch each other in this manner. I asked a gentleman, who had kept foxhounds all his life, and be
applied to other experienced sportsmen, whether they had ever seen hounds thus seize a fox; but they never
had. It appears that when a dog is chased, or when in danger of being struck behind, or of anything falling on
him, in all these cases he wishes to withdraw as quickly as possible his whole hindquarters, and that from
some sympathy or connection between the muscles, the tail is then drawn closely inwards. A similarly
connected movement between the hind quarters and the tail may be observed in the hyaena. Mr. Bartlett
informs me that when two of these animals fight together, they are mutually conscious of the wonderful
power of each other's jaws, and are extremely cautious. They well know that if one of their legs were seized,
the bone would instantly be crushed into atoms; hence they approach each other kneeling, with their legs
turned as much as possible inwards, and with their whole bodies bowed, so as not to present any salient point;
thetail at the same time being closely tucked in between the legs. In this attitude they approach each other
sideways, or even partly backwards. So again with deer, several of the species, when savage and fighting,
tuck in their tails. When one horse in a field tries to bite the hindquarters of another in play, or when a rough
boy strikes a donkey from behind, the hindquarters and the tail are drawn in, though it does not appear as if
this were done merely to save the tail from being injured. We have also seen the reverse of these movements;
for when an animal trots with high elastic steps, the tail is almost always carried aloft. As I have said, when a
dog is chased and runs away, he keeps his ears directed backwards but still open; and this is clearly done for
the sake of hearing the footsteps of his pursuer. From habit the ears are often held in this same position, and
the tail tucked in, when the danger is obviously in front. I have repeatedly noticed, with a timid terrier of
mine, that when she is afraid of some object in front, the nature of which she perfectly knows and does not
need to reconnoitre, yet she will for a long time hold her ears and tail in this position, looking the image of
discomfort. Discomfort, without any fear, is similarly expressed: thus, one day I went out of doors, just at the
time when this same dog knew that her dinner would be brought. I did not call her, but she wished much to
accompany me, and at the same time she wished much for her dinner; and there she stood, first looking one
way and then the other, with her tail tucked in and ears drawn back, presenting an unmistakable appearance
of perplexed discomfort. Almost all the expressive movements now described, with the exception of the
grinning from joy, are innate or instinctive, for they are common to all the individuals, young and old, of all
the breeds. Most of themare likewise common to the aboriginal parents of the dog, namely the wolf and
jackal; and some of them to other species of the same group. Tamed wolves and jackals, when caressed by
their masters, jump about for joy, wag their tails, lower their ears, lick their master's hands, crouch down, and
even throw themselves on the ground belly upwards.[4] I have seen a rather foxlike African jackal, from the
Gaboon, depress its ears when caressed. Wolves and jackals, when frightened, certainly tuck in their tails;
and a tamed jackal has been described as careering round his master in circles and figures of eight, like a dog,
with his tail between his legs. It has been stated[5] that foxes, however tame, never display any of the above
expressive movements; but this is not strictly accurate. Many years ago I observed in the Zoological Gardens,
and recorded the fact at the time, that a very tame English fox, When caressed by the keeper, wagged its tail,
depressed its ears, and then threw itself on the ground, belly upwards. The black fox of North America
likewise depressed its ears in a slight degree. But I believe that foxes never lick the hands of their masters,
and I have been assured that when frightened they never tuck in their tails. If the explanation which I have
given of the expression of affection in dogs be admitted, then it would appear that animals which have never
been domesticatednamely wolves, jackals, and even foxes have nevertheless ac quired, through the
principle of antithesis, certain expressive gestures; for it is Dot probable that these animals, confined in cages,
should have learnt them by imitating dogs. [4] Many particulars are given by Gueldenstadt in his account of
the jackal in Nov. Comm. Acad. Sc. Imp. Petrop. 1775, tom. xx. p. 449. See also another excellent account of
the manners of this animal and of its play, in `Land and Water,' October, 1869. Lieut. Annesley, R. A., has
also communicated to me some particulars with respect to the jackal. I have made many inquiries about
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 47
Page No 50
wolves and jackals in the Zoological Gardens, and have observed them for myself. [5] `Land and Water,'
November 6, 1869._Cats_.I have already described the actions of a cat
(fig. 9), when feeling savage and not terrified. She assumes a crouching attitude and occasionally protrudes
her forefeet, with the claws exserted ready for striking. The tail is extended, being curled or lashed from
side to side. The hair is not erectedat least it was not so in the few cases observed by me. The ears are
drawn closely backwards and the teeth are shown. Low savage growls are uttered. We can understand why
the attitude assumed by a cat when preparing to fight with another cat, or in any way greatly irritated, is so
widely different from that of a dog approaching another dog with hostile intentions; for the cat uses her
forefeet for striking, and this renders a crouching position convenient or necessary. She is also much more
accustomed than a dog to lie concealed and suddenly spring on her prey. No cause can be assigned with
certainty for the tail being lashed or curled from side to side. This habit is common to many other
animalsfor instance, to the puma, when prepared to spring;[1] but it is not common to dogs, or to foxes, as
I infer from Mr. St. John's account of a fox lying in wait and seizing a hare. We have already seen that some
kinds of lizards and various snakes, when excited, rapidly vibrate the tips of their tails. It would appear as if,
under strong excitement, there existed an uncontrollable desire for movement of some kind, owing to
nerveforce being freely liberated from the excited sensorium; and that as the tail is left free, and as its
movement does not disturb the general position of the body, it is curled or lashed about.
All the movements of a cat, when feeling affectionate, are in complete antithesis to those just described. She
now stands upright, with slightly arched back, tail perpendicularly raised, and ears erected; and she rubs her
cheeks and flanks against her master or mistress. The desire to rub something is so strong in cats under this
state of mind, that they may often be seen rubbing themselves against the legs of chairs or tables, or against
doorposts. This manner of expressing affection probably originated through association, as in the case of
dogs, from the mother nursing and fondling her young; and perhaps from the young themselves loving each
other and playing together. Another and very different gesture, expressive of pleasure, has already been
described, namely, the curious manner in which young and even old cats, when pleased, alternately protrude
their forefeet, with separated toes, as if pushing against and sucking their mother's teats. This habit is so far
analogous to that of rubbing against something, that both apparently are derived from actions performed
during the nursing period. Why cats should show affection by rubbing so much more than do dogs, though
the latter delight in contact with their masters, and why cats only occasionally lick the hands of their friends,
whilst dogs always do so, I cannot say. Cats cleanse themselves by licking their own coats more regularly
than do dogs. On the other hand, their tongues seem less well fitted for the work than the longer and more
flexible tongues of dogs.
[1] Azara, `Quadrupedes du Paraquay,' 1801, tom. 1. p. 136.
Cats, when terrified, stand at full height, and arch their backs in a wellknown and ridiculous fashion. They
spit, hiss, or growl. The hair over the whole body, and especially on the tail, becomes erect. In the instances
observed by me the basal part of the tail was held upright, the terminal part being thrown on one side; but
sometimes the tail (see fig. 15) is only a little raised, and is bent almost from the base to one side. The ears
are drawn back, and the teeth exposed. When two kittens are playing together, the one often thus tries to
frighten the other. From what we have seen in former chapters, all the above points of expression are
intelligible, except the extreme arching of the back. I am inclined to believe that, in the same manner as many
birds, whilst they ruffle their feathers, spread out their wings and tail, to make themselves look as big as
possible, so cats stand upright at their full height, arch their backs, often raise the basal part of the tail, and
erect their hair, for the same purpose. The lynx, when attacked, is said to arch its back, and is thus figured by
Brehm. But the keepers in the Zoological Gardens have never seen any tendency to this action in the larger
feline animals, such as tigers, lions, and these have little cause to be afraid of any other animal.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 48
Page No 51
Cats use their voices much as a means of expression, and they utter, under various emotions and desires, at
least six or seven different sounds. The purr of satisfaction, which is made during both inspiration and
expiration, is one of the most curious. The puma, cheetah, and ocelot likewise purr; but the tiger, when
pleased, "emits a peculiar short snuffle, accompanied by the closure of the eyelids."[7] It is said that the lion,
jaguar, and leopard, do not purr.
_Horses_.Horses when savage draw their ears closely back, protrude their heads, and partially uncover
their incisor teeth, ready for biting. When inclined to kick behind, they generally, through habit, draw back
their ears; and their eyes are turned backwards in a peculiar manner.[8] When pleased, as when some coveted
food is brought to them in the stable, they raise and draw in their heads, prick their ears, and looking intently
towards their friend, often whinny. Impatience is expressed by pawing the ground.
[7] `Land and Water,' 1867, p. 657. See also Azara on the Puma, in the work above quoted.
[8] Sir C. Bell, `Anatomy of Expression,' 3rd edit. p. 123. See also p. 126, on horses not breathing through
their mouths, with reference to their distended nostrils.
The actions of a horse when much startled are highly expressive. One day my horse was much frightened at a
drilling machine, covered by a tarpaulin, and lying on an open field. He raised his head so high, that his neck
became almost perpendicular; and this he did from habit, for the machine lay on a slope below, and could not
have been seen with more distinctness through the raising of the head; nor if any sound had proceeded from
it, could the sound have been more distinctly heard. His eyes and ears were directed intently forwards; and I
could feel through the saddle the palpitations of his heart. With red dilated nostrils he snorted violently, and
whirling round, would have dashed off at full speed, had I not prevented him. The distension of the nostrils is
not for the sake of scenting the source of danger, for when a horse smells carefully at any object and is not
alarmed, he does not dilate his nostrils. Owing to the presence of a valve in the throat, a horse when panting
does not breathe through his open mouth, but through his nostrils; and these consequently have become
endowed with great powers of expansion. This expansion of the nostrils, as well as the snorting, and the
palpitations of the heart, are actions which have become firmly associated during a long series of generations
with the emotion of terror; for terror has habitually led the horse to the most violent exertion in dashing away
at full speed from the cause of danger.
_Ruminants_.Cattle and sheep are remarkable from displaying in so slight a degree their emotions or
sensations, excepting that of extreme pain. A bull when enraged exhibits his rage only by the manner in
which be holds his lowered head, with distended nostrils, and by bellowing. He also often paws the ground;
but this pawing seems quite different from that of an impatient horse, for when the soil is loose, he throws up
clouds of dust. I believe that bulls act in this manner when irritated by flies, for the sake of driving them
away. The wilder breeds of sheep and the chamois when startled stamp on the ground, and whistle through
their noses; and this serves as a dangersignal to their comrades. The muskox of the Arctic regions, when
encountered, likewise stamps on the ground.[9] How this stamping action arose I cannot conjecture; for from
inquiries which I have made it does not appear that any of these animals fight with their forelegs.
Some species of deer, when savage, display far more expression than do cattle, sheep, or goats, for, as has
already been stated, they draw back their ears, grind their teeth, erect their hair, squeal, stamp on the ground,
and brandish their horns. One day in the Zoological Gardens, the Formosan deer (_Cervus pseudaxis_)
approached me in a curious attitude, with his muzzle raised high up, so that the horns were pressed back on
his neck; the head being held rather obliquely. From the expression of his eye I felt sure that he was savage;
he approached slowly, and as soon as he came close to the iron bars, he did not lower his head to butt at me,
but suddenly bent it inwards, and struck his horns with great force against the railings. Mr. Bartlett informs
me that some other species of deer place themselves in the same attitude when enraged.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 49
Page No 52
_Monkeys_.The various species and genera of monkeys express their feelings in many different ways; and
this fact is interesting, as in some degree bearing on the question, whether the socalled races of man should
be ranked as distinct species or varieties; for, as we shall see in the following chapters, the different races of
man express their emotions and sensations with remarkable uniformity throughout the world. Some of the
expressive actions of monkeys are interesting in another way, namely from being closely analogous to those
of man. As I have had no opportunity of observing any one species of the group under all circumstances, my
miscellaneous remarks will be best arranged under different states of the mind.
[9] `Land and Water,' 1869, p. 152.
_Pleasure, joy, affection_It is not possible to distinguish in monkeys, at least without more experience than
I have had, the expression of pleasure or joy from that of affection. Young chimpanzees make a kind of
barking noise, when pleased by the return of any one to whom they are attached. When this noise, which the
keepers call a laugh, is uttered, the lips are protruded; but so they are under various other emotions.
Nevertheless I could perceive that when they were pleased the form of the lips differed a little from that
assumed when they were angered. If a young chimpanzee be tickled and the armpits are particularly
sensitive to tickling, as in the case of our children,a more decided chuckling or laughing sound is uttered;
though the laughter is sometimes noiseless. The corners of the mouth are then drawn backwards; and this
sometimes causes the lower eyelids to be slightly wrinkled. But this wrinkling, which is so characteristic of
our own laughter, is more plainly seen in some other monkeys. The teeth in the upper jaw in the chimpanzee
are not exposed when they utter their laughing noise, in which respect they differ from us. But their eyes
sparkle and grow brighter, as Mr. W. L. Martin,[10] who has particularly attended to their expression, states.
[10] `Natural History of Mammalia,' 1841, vol. 1. pp. 383, 410.
Young Orangs, when tickled, likewise grin and make a chuckling sound; and Mr. Martin says that their eyes
grow brighter. As soon as their laughter ceases, an expression may be detected passing over their faces,
which, as Mr. Wallace remarked to me, may be called a smile. I have also noticed something of the same
kind with the chimpanzee. Dr. Duchenneand I cannot quote a better authorityinforms me that he kept a
very tame monkey in his house for a year; and when he gave it during mealtimes some choice delicacy, he
observed that the corners of its mouth were slightly raised; thus an expression of satisfaction, partaking of the
nature of an incipient smile, and resembling that often seen on the face of main, could be plainly perceived in
this animal.
The _Cebus azarae_,[11] when rejoiced at again seeing a beloved person, utters a peculiar tittering
(_kichernden_) sound. It also expresses agreeable sensations, by drawing back the corners of its mouth,
without producing any sound. Rengger calls this movement laughter, but it would be more appropriately
called a smile. The form of the mouth is different when either pain or terror is expressed, and high shrieks are
uttered. Another species of _Cebus_ in the Zoological Gardens (_C. hypoleucus_) when pleased, makes a
reiterated shrill note, and likewise draws back the corners of its mouth, apparently through the contraction of
the same muscles as with us. So does the Barbary ape (_Inuus ecaudatus_) to an extraordinary degree; and I
observed in this monkey that the skin of the lower eyelids then became much wrinkled. At the same time it
rapidly moved its lower jaw or lips in a spasmodic manner, the teeth being exposed; but the noise produced
was hardly more distinct than that which we sometimes call silent laughter. Two of the keepers affirmed that
this slight sound was the animal's laughter, and when I expressed some doubt on this head (being at the time
quite inexperienced), they made it attack or rather threaten a hated Entellus monkey, living in the same
compartment. Instantly the whole expression of the face of the Inuus changed; the mouth was opened much
more widely, the canine teeth were more fully exposed, and a hoarse barking noise was uttered.
[11] Rengger (`Sagetheire von Paraquay', 1830, s. 46) kept these monkeys in confinement for seven years in
their native country of Paraguay.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 50
Page No 53
The Anubis baboon (_Cynocephalus anubis_) was first insulted and put into a furious rage, as was easily
done, by his keeper, who then made friends with him and shook hands. As the reconciliation was effected the
baboon rapidly moved up and down his jaws and lips, and looked pleased. When we laugh heartily, a similar
movement, or quiver, may be observed more or less distinctly in our jaws; but with man the muscles of the
chest are more particularly acted on, whilst with this baboon, and with some other monkeys, it is the muscles
of the jaws and lips which are spasmodically affected.
I have already had occasion to remark on the curious manner in which two or three species of Alacacus and
the _Cynopithecus niger_ draw back their ears and utter a slight jabbering noise, when they are pleased by
being caressed. With the Cynopithecus (fig. 17), the corners of the mouth are at the same time drawn
backwards and upwards, so that the teeth are exposed. Hence this expression would never be recognized by a
stranger as one of pleasure. The crest of long hairs on the forehead is depressed, and apparently the whole
skin of the head drawn backwards. The eyebrows are thus raised a little, and the eyes assume a staring
appearance. The lower eyelids also become slightly wrinkled; but this wrinkling is not conspicuous, owing to
the permanent transverse furrows on the face.
_Painful emotions and sensations_.With monkeys the expression of slight pain, or of any painful emotion,
such as grief, vexation, jealousy, is not easily distinguished from that of moderate anger; and these states of
mind readily and quickly pass into each other. Grief, however, with some species is certainly exhibited by
weeping. A woman, who sold a monkey to the Zoological Society, believed to have come from Borneo
(_Macacus maurus_ or _M. inornatus_ of Gray), said that it often cried; and Mr. Bartlett, as well as the
keeper Mr. Sutton, have repeatedly seen it, when grieved, or even when much pitied, weeping so copiously
that the tears rolled down its cheeks. There is, however, something strange about this case, for two specimens
subsequently kept in the Gardens, and believed to be the same species, have never been seen to weep, though
they were carefully observed by the keeper and myself when much distressed and loudly screaming. Rengger
states[12] that the eyes of the _Cebus azarae_ fill with tears, but not sufficiently to overflow, when it is
prevented getting some much desired object, or is much frightened. Humboldt also asserts that the eyes of the
_Callithrix sciureus_ "instantly fill with tears when it is seized with fear;" but when this pretty little monkey
in the Zoological Gardens was teased, so as to cry out loudly, this did not occur. I do not, however, wish to
throw the least doubt on the accuracy of Humboldt's statement.
The appearance of dejection in young orangs and chimpanzees, when out of health, is as plain and almost as
pathetic as in the case of our children. This state of mind and body is shown by their listless movements,
fallen countenances, dull eyes, and changed complexion.
[12] Rengger, ibid. s. 46. Humboldt, `Personal Narrative, Eng. translat. vol. iv. p. 527. {Illust. caption = FIG.
16._Cynopithecus niger_, in a placid condition.
Drawn from life by Mr. Wolf. FIG. 17.The same, when pleased by being caressed.}
_Anger_.This emotion is often exhibited by many kinds of monkeys, and is expressed, as Mr. Martin
remarks,[13] in many different ways. "Some species, when irritated, pout the lips, gaze with a fixed and
savage glare on their foe, and make repeated short starts as if about to spring forward, uttering at the same
time inward guttural sounds. Many display their anger by suddenly advancing, making abrupt starts, at the
same time opening the mouth and pursing up the lips, so as to conceal the teeth, while the eyes are daringly
fixed on the enemy, as if in savage defiance. Some again, and principally the longtailed monkeys, or
Guenons, display their teeth, and accompany their malicious grins with a sharp, abrupt, reiterated cry." Mr.
Sutton confirms the statement that some species uncover their teeth when enraged, whilst others conceal them
by the protrusion of their lips; and some kinds draw back their ears. The _Cynopithecus niger_, lately referred
to, acts in this manner, at the same time depressing the crest of hair on its forehead, and showing its teeth; so
that the movements of the features from anger are nearly the same as those from pleasure; and the two
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 51
Page No 54
expressions can be distinguished only by those familiar with the animal.
Baboons often show their passion and threaten their enemies in a very odd manner, namely, by opening their
mouths widely as in the act of yawning. Mr. Bartlett has often seen two baboons, when first placed in the
same compartment, sitting opposite to each other and thus alternately opening their mouths; and this action
seems frequently to end in a real yawn. Mr. Bartlett believes that both animals wish to show to each other that
they are provided with a formidable set of teeth, as is undoubtedly the case. As I could hardly credit the
reality of this yawning gesture, Mr. Bartlett insulted an old baboon and put him into a violent passion; and he
almost immediately thus acted. Some species of Macacus and of Cereopithecus[14] behave in the same
manner. Baboons likewise show their anger, as was observed by Brehin with those which he kept alive in
Abyssinia, in another manner, namely, by striking the ground with one hand, "like an angry man striking the
table with his fist." I have seen this movement with the baboons in the Zoological Gardens; but sometimes
the action seems rather to represent the searching for a stone or other object in their beds of straw.
[13] Nat. Hist. of Mammalia, 1841, p. 351.
Mr. Sutton has often observed the face of the _Macacus rhesus_, when much enraged, growing red. As he
was mentioning this to me, another monkey attacked a _rhesus_, and I saw its face redden as plainly as that
of a man in a violent passion. In the course of a few minutes, after the battle, the face of this monkey
recovered its natural tint. At the same time that the face reddened, the naked posterior part of the body, which
is always red, seemed to grow still redder; but I cannot positively assert that this was the case. When the
Mandrill is in any way excited, the brilliantly coloured, naked parts of the skin are said to become still more
vividly coloured.
With several species of baboons the ridge of the forehead projects much over the eyes, and is studded with a
few long hairs, representing our eyebrows. These animals are always looking about them, and in order to look
upwards they raise their eyebrows. They have thus, as it would appear, acquired the habit of frequently
moving their eyebrows. However this may be, many kinds of monkeys, especially the baboons, when angered
or in any way excited, rapidly and incessantly move their eyebrows up and down, as well as the hairy skin of
their foreheads.[15] As we associate in the case of man the raising and lowering of the eyebrows with definite
states of the mind, the almost incessant movement of the eyebrows by monkeys gives them a senseless
expression. I once observed a man who had a trick of continually raising his eyebrows without any
corresponding emotion, and this gave to him a foolish appearance; so it is with some persons who keep the
corners of their mouths a little drawn backwards and upwards, as if by an incipient smile, though at the time
they are not amused or pleased.
[14] Brehm, `Thierleben,' B. i. s. 84. On baboons striking the ground, s. 61.
A young orang, made jealous by her keeper attending to another monkey, slightly uncovered her teeth, and,
uttering a peevish noise like _tishshist_, turned her back on him. Both orangs and chimpanzees, when a little
more angered, protrude their lips greatly, and make a harsh barking noise. A young female chimpanzee, in a
violent passion, presented a curious resemblance to a child in the same state. She screamed loudly with
widely open mouth, the lips being retracted so that the teeth were fully exposed. She threw her arms wildly
about, sometimes clasping them over her head. She rolled on the ground, sometimes on her back, sometimes
on her belly, and bit everything within reach. A young gibbon (_Hylobates syndactylus_) in a passion has
been described[16] as behaving in almost exactly the same manner.
The lips of young orangs and chimpanzees are protruded, sometimes to a wonderful degree, under various
circumstances. They act thus, not only when slightly angered, sulky, or disappointed, but when alarmed at
anythingin one instance, at the sight of a turtle,[17]and likewise when pleased. But neither the degree of
protrusion nor the shape of the mouth is exactly the same, as I believe, in all cases; and the sounds which are
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 52
Page No 55
then uttered are different. The accompanying drawing represents a chimpanzee made sulky by an orange
having been offered him, and then taken away. A similar protrusion or pouting of the lips, though to a much
slighter degree, may be seen in sulky children.
[15] Brehm remarks (`Thierleben,' s. 68) that the eyebrows of the _Inuus ecaudatus_ are frequently moved up
and down when the animal is angered.
[16] G. Bennett, `Wanderings in New South Wales,' vol. ii. 1834, p. 153. FIG. 18.Chimpanzee disappointed
and sulky. Drawn from life by Mr. Wood.
Many years ago, in the Zoological Gardens, I placed a lookingglass on the floor before two young orangs,
who, as far as it was known, had never before seen one. At first they gazed at their own images with the most
steady surprise, and often changed their point of view. They then approached close and protruded their lips
towards the image, as if to kiss it, in exactly the same manner as they had previously done towards each
other, when first placed, a few days before, in the same room. They next made all sorts of grimaces, and put
themselves in various attitudes before the mirror; they pressed and rubbed the surface; they placed their hands
at different distances behind it; looked behind it; and finally seemed almost frightened, started a little, became
cross, and refused to look any longer.
When we try to perform some little action which is difficult and requires precision, for instance, to thread a
needle, we generally close our lips firmly, for the sake, I presume, of not disturbing our movements by
breathing; and I noticed the same action in a young Orang. The poor little creature was sick, and was amusing
itself by trying to kill the flies on the windowpanes with its knuckles; this was difficult as the flies buzzed
about, and at each attempt the lips were firmly compressed, and at the same time slightly protruded.
[17] W. L. Martin, Nat. Hist. of Mamm. Animals, 1841, p. 405.
Although the countenances, and more especially the gestures, of orangs and chimpanzees are in some
respects highly expressive, I doubt whether on the whole they are so expressive as those of some other kinds
of monkeys. This may be attributed in part to their ears being immovable, and in part to the nakedness of
their eyebrows, of which the movements are thus rendered less conspicuous. When, however, they raise their
eyebrows their foreheads become, as with us, transversely wrinkled. In comparison with man, their faces are
inexpressive, chiefly owing to their not frowning under any emotion of the mindthat is, as far as I have
been able to observe, and I carefully attended to this point. Frowning, which is one of the most important of
all the expressions in man, is due to the contraction of the corrugators by which the eyebrows are lowered and
brought together, so that vertical furrows are formed on the forehead. Both the orang and chimpanzee are
said[18] to possess this muscle, but it seems rarely brought into action, at least in a conspicuous manner. I
made my hands into a sort of cage, and placing some tempting fruit within, allowed both a young orang and
chimpanzee to try their utmost to get it out; but although they grew rather cross, they showed not a trace of a
frown. Nor was there any frown when they were enraged. Twice I took two chimpanzees from their rather
dark room suddenly into bright sunshine, which would certainly have caused us to frown; they blinked and
winked their eyes, but only once did I see a very slight frown. On another occasion, I tickled the nose of a
chimpanzee with a straw, and as it crumpled up its face, slight vertical furrows appeared between the
eyebrows. I have never seen a frown on the forehead of the orang.
[18] Prof. Owen on the Orang, Proc. Zool. Soc. 1830, p. 28. On the Chimpanzee, see Prof. Macalister, in
Annals and Mag. of Nat. Hist. vol. vii. 1871, p. 342, who states that the _corrugator supercilii_ is inseparable
from the _orbicularis palpebrarum_.
The gorilla, when enraged, is described as erecting its crest of hair, throwing down its under lip, dilating its
nostrils, and uttering terrific yells. Messrs. Savage and Wyman[19] state that the scalp can be freely moved
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 53
Page No 56
backwards and forwards, and that when the animal is excited it is strongly contracted; but I presume that they
mean by this latter expression that the scalp is lowered; for they likewise speak of the young chimpanzee,
when crying out, as having the eyebrows strongly contracted." The great power of movement in the scalp of
the gorilla, of many baboons and other monkeys, deserves notice in relation to the power possessed by some
few men, either through reversion or persistence, of voluntarily moving their scalps.[20]
_Astonishment, Terror_A living freshwater turtle was placed at my request in the same compartment in
the Zoological Gardens with many monkeys; and they showed unbounded astonishment, as well as some fear.
This was displayed by their remaining motionless, staring intently with widely opened eyes, their eyebrows
being often moved up and down. Their faces seemed somewhat lengthened. They occasionally raised
themselves on their hindlegs to get abetter view. They often retreated a few feet, and then turning their
heads over one shoulder, again stared intently. It was curious to observe how much less afraid they were of
the turtle than of a living snake which I had formerly placed in their compartment;[21] for in the course of a
few minutes some of the monkeys ventured to approach and touch the turtle. On the other hand, some of the
larger baboons were greatly terrified, and grinned as if on the point of screaming out. When I showed a little
dressedup doll to the _Cynopithecus niger_, it stood motionless, stared intently with widely opened eyes,
and advanced its ears a little forwards. But when the turtle was placed in its compartment, this monkey also
moved its lips in an odd, rapid, jabbering manner, which the keeper declared was meant to conciliate or
please the turtle.
[19] Boston Journal of Nat. Hist. 184547, vol. v. p. 423. On the Chimpanzee, ibid. 184344, vol. iv. p.
365.
[20] See on this subject, `Descent of Man,' vol. i. p. 20.
I was never able clearly to perceive that the eyebrows of astonished monkeys were kept permanently raised,
though they were frequently moved up and down. Attention, which precedes astonishment, is expressed by
man by a slight raising of the eyebrows; and Dr. Duchenne informs me that when he gave to the monkey
formerly mentioned some quite new article of food, it elevated its eyebrows a little, thus assuming an
appearance of close attention. It then took the food in its fingers, and, with lowered or rectilinear eyebrows,
scratched, smelt, and examined it, an expression of reflection being thus exhibited. Sometimes it would
throw back its head a little, and again with suddenly raised eyebrows reexamine and finally taste the food.
In no case did any monkey keep its mouth open when it was astonished. Mr. Sutton observed for me a young
orang and chimpanzee during a considerable length of time; and however much they were astonished, or
whilst listening intently to some strange sound, they did not keep their mouths open. This fact is surprising,
as with mankind hardly any expression is more general than a widely open mouth under the sense of
astonishment. As far as I have been able to observe, monkeys breathe more freely through their nostrils than
men do; and this may account for their not opening their mouths when they are astonished; for, as we shall
see in a future chapter, man apparently acts in this manner when startled, at first for the sake of quickly
drawing a full inspiration, and afterwards for the sake of breathing as quietly as possible.
[21] `Descent of Man,' vol, i. p, 43.
Terror is expressed by many kinds of monkeys by the utterance of shrill screams; the lips being drawn back,
so that the teeth are exposed. The hair becomes erect, especially when some anger is likewise felt. Mr. Sutton
has distinctly seen the face of the _Macacus rhesus_ grow pale from fear. Monkeys also tremble from fear;
and sometimes they void their excretions. I have seen one which, when caught, almost fainted from an excess
of terror.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS. 54
Page No 57
Sufficient facts have now been given with respect to the expressions of various animals. It is impossible to
agree with Sir C. Bell when he says[22] that "the faces of animals seem chiefly capable of expressing rage
and fear;" and again, when he says that all their expressions "may be referred, more or less plainly, to their
acts of volition or necessary instincts." He who will look at a dog preparing to attack another dog or a man,
and at the same animal when caressing his master, or will watch the countenance of a monkey when insulted,
and when fondled by his keeper, will be forced to admit that the movements of their features and their
gestures are almost as expressive as those of man. Although no explanation can be given of some of the
expressions in the lower animals, the greater number are explicable in accordance with the three principles
given at the commencement of the first chapter.
[22] `Anatomy of Expression,' 3rd edit. 1844, pp. 138, 121.
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND
WEEPING.
The screaming and weeping Of infantsForms of features Age at which weeping commencesThe
effects of habitual restraint on weepingSobbingCause of the contraction of the muscles round the eyes
during screamingCause of the secretion of tears.
IN this and the following chapters the expressions exhibited by Man under various states of the mind will be
described and explained, as far as lies in my power. My observations will be arranged according to the order
which I have found the most convenient; and this will generally lead to opposite emotions and sensations
succeeding each other.
_Suffering of the body and mind: weeping_.I have already described in sufficient detail, in the third
chapter, the signs of extreme pain, as shown by screams or groans, with the writhing of the whole body and
the teeth clenched or ground together. These signs are often accompanied or followed by profuse sweating,
pallor, trembling, utter prostration, or faintness. No suffering is greater than that from extreme fear or horror,
but here a distinct emotion comes into play, and will be elsewhere considered. Prolonged suffering, especially
of the mind, passes into low spirits, grief, dejection, and despair, and these states will be the subject of the
following chapter. Here I shall almost confine myself to weeping or crying, more especially in children.
Infants, when suffering even slight pain, moderate hunger, or discomfort, utter violent and prolonged
screams. Whilst thus screaming their eyes are firmly closed, so that the skin round them is wrinkled, and the
forehead contracted into a frown. The mouth is widely opened with the lips retracted in a peculiar manner,
which causes it to assume a squarish form; the gums or teeth being more or less exposed. The breath is
inhaled almost spasmodically. It is easy to observe infants whilst screaming; but I have found photographs
made by the instantaneous process the best means for observation, as allowing more deliberation. I have
collected twelve, most of them made purposely for me; and they all exhibit the same general characteristics. I
have, therefore, had six of them[1] (Plate I.) reproduced by the heliotype process.
The firm closing of the eyelids and consequent compression of the eyeball,and this is a most important
element in various expressions,serves to protect the eyes from becoming too much gorged with blood, as
will presently be explained in detail. With respect to the order in which the several muscles contract in firmly
compressing the eyes, I am indebted to Dr. Langstaff, of Southampton, for some observations, which I have
since repeated. The best plan for observing the order is to make a person first raise his eyebrows, and this
produces transverse wrinkles across the forehead; and then very gradually to contract all the muscles round
the elves with as much force as possible. The reader who is unacquainted with the anatomy of the face, ought
to refer to p. 24, and look at the woodcuts 1 to 3. The corrugators of the brow (_corrugator supercilii_) seem
to be the first muscles to contract; and these draw the eyebrows downwards and inwards towards the base of
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 55
Page No 58
the nose, causing vertical furrows, that is a frown, to appear between the eyebrows; at the same time they
cause the disappearance of the transverse wrinkles across the forehead. The orbicular muscles contract almost
simultaneously with the corrugators, and produce wrinkles all round the eyes; they appear, however, to be
enabled to contract with greater force, as soon as the contraction of the corrugators has given them some
support. Lastly, the pyramidal muscles of the nose contract; and these draw the eyebrows and the skin of the
forehead still lower down, producing short transverse wrinkles across the base of the nose.[2] For the sake of
brevity these muscles will generally be spoken of as the orbiculars, or as those surrounding the eyes.
[1] The best photographs in my collection are by Mr. Rejlander, of Victoria Street, London, and by Herr
Kindermann, of Hamburg. Figs. 1, 3, 4, and 6 are by the former; and figs. 2 and 5, by the latter gentleman.
Fig. 6 is given to show moderate crying in an older child.
When these muscles are strongly contracted, those running to the upper lip[3] likewise contract and raise the
upper lip. This might have been expected from the manner in which at least one of them, the _malaris_, is
connected with the orbiculars. Any one who will gradually contract the muscles round his eyes, will feel, as
he increases the force, that his upper lip and the wings of his nose (which are partly acted on by one of the
same muscles) are almost always a little drawn up. If he keeps his mouth firmly shut whilst contracting the
muscles round the eyes, and then suddenly relaxes his lips, he will feel that the pressure on his eyes
immediately increases. So again when a person on a bright, glaring day wishes to look at a distant object, but
is compelled partially to close his eyelids, the upper lip may almost always be observed to be somewhat
raised. The mouths of some very shortsighted persons, who are forced habitually to reduce the aperture of
their eyes, wear from this same reason a grinning expression.
[2] Henle (`Handbuch d. Syst. Anat. 1858, B. i. s. 139) agrees with Duchenne that this is the effect of the
contraction of the _pyramidalis nasi_.
[3] These consist of the _levator labii superioris alaeque nasi_, the _levator labii proprius_, the _malaris_, and
the _zygomaticus minor_, or little zygomatic. This latter muscle runs parallel to and above the great
zygomatic, and is attached to the outer part of the upper lip. It is represented in fig. 2 (I. p. 24), but not in figs.
1 and 3. Dr. Duchenne first showed (`Mecanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,' Album, 1862, p. 39) the
importance of the contraction of this muscle in the shape assumed by the features in crying. Henle considers
the abovenamed muscles (excepting the _malaris_) as subdivisions of the q_uadratus labii superioris_.
The raising of the upper lip draws upwards the flesh of the upper parts of the cheeks, and produces a strongly
marked fold on each cheek,the nasolabial fold,which runs from near the wings of the nostrils to the
corners of the mouth and below them. This fold or furrow may be seen in all the photographs, and is very
characteristic of the expression of a crying child; though a nearly similar fold is produced in the act of
laughing or Smiling.[4]
[4] Although Dr. Duchenne has so carefully studied the contraction of the different muscles during the act of
crying, and the furrows on the face thus produced, there seems to be something incomplete in his account; but
what this is I cannot say. He has given a figure (Album, fig. 48) in which one half of the face is made, by
galvanizing the proper muscles, to smile; whilst the other half is similarly made to begin crying. Almost all
those (viz. nineteen out of twentyone persons) to whom I showed the smiling half of the face instantly
recognized the expression; but, with respect to the other half, only six persons out of twentyone recognized
it,that is, if we accept such terms as "grief," "misery," "annoyance," as correct;whereas, fifteen persons
were ludicrously mistaken; some of them saying the face expressed "fun," "satisfaction," "cunning,"
"disgust," We may infer from this that there is something wrong in the expression. Some of the fifteen
persons may, however, have been partly misled by not expecting to see an old man crying, and by tears not
being secreted. With respect to another figure by Dr. Duchenne (fig. 49), in which the muscles of half the
face are galvanized in order to represent a man beginning to cry, with the eyebrow on the same side rendered
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 56
Page No 59
oblique, which is characteristic of misery, the expression was recognized by a greater proportional number of
persons. Out of twentythree persons, fourteen answered correctly, "sorrow," "distress," "grief," "just going
to cry," "endurance of pain," On the other hand, nine persons either could form no opinion or were entirely
wrong, answering, "cunning leer," "jocund," "looking at an intense light," "looking at a distant object,"
As the upper lip is much drawn up during the act of screaming, in the manner just explained, the depressor
muscles of the angles of the mouth (see K in woodcuts 1 and 2) are strongly contracted in order to keep the
mouth widely open, so that a full volume of sound may be poured forth. The action of these opposed muscles,
above and below, tends to give to the mouth an oblong, almost squarish outline, as may be seen in the
accompanying photographs. An excellent observer,[5] in describing a baby crying whilst being fed, says, "it
made its mouth like a square, and let the porridge run out at all four corners." I believe, but we shall return to
this point in a future chapter, that the depressor muscles of the angles of the mouth are less under the separate
control of the will than the adjoining muscles; so that if a young child is only doubtfully inclined to cry, this
muscle is generally the first to contract, and is the last to cease contracting. When older children commence
crying, the muscles which run to the upper lip are often the first to contract; and this may perhaps be due to
older children not having so strong a tendency to scream loudly, and consequently to keep their mouths
widely open; so that the abovenamed depressor muscles are not brought into such strong action.
[5] Mrs. Gaskell, `Mary Barton,' new edit. p. 84.
With one of my own infants, from his eighth day and for some time afterwards, I often observed that the first
sign of a screamingfit, when it could be observed coming on gradually, was a little frown, owing to the
contraction of the corrugators of the brows; the capillaries of the naked head and face becoming at the same
time reddened with blood. As soon as the screamingfit actually began, all the muscles round the eyes were
strongly contracted, and the mouth widely opened in the manlier above described; so that at this early period
the features assumed the same form as at a more advanced age.
Dr. Piderit[6] lays great stress on the contraction of certain muscles which draw down the nose and narrow
the nostrils, as eminently characteristic of a crying expression. The _depressores anguli oris_, as we have just
seen, are usually contracted at the same time, and they indirectly tend, according to Dr. Duchenne, to act in
this same manner on the nose. With children having bad colds a similar pinched appearance of the nose may
be noticed, which is at least partly due, as remarked to me by Dr. Langstaff, to their constant snuffling, and
the consequent pressure of the atmosphere on the two sides. The purpose of this contraction of the nostrils by
children having bad colds, or whilst crying, seems to be to cheek the downward flow of the mucus and tears,
and to prevent these fluids spreading over the upper lip.
After a prolonged and severe screamingfit, the scalp, face, and eyes are reddened, owing to the return of the
blood from the head having been impeded by the violent expiratory efforts; but the redness of the stimulated
eyes is chiefly due to the copious effusion of tears. The various muscles of the face which have been strongly
contracted, still twitch a little, and the upper lip is still slightly drawn up or everted,[7] with the corners of the
mouth still a little drawn downwards. I have myself felt, and have observed in other grownup persons, that
when tears are restrained with difficulty, as in reading a pathetic story, it is almost impossible to prevent the
various muscles. which with young children are brought into strong action during their screamingfits, from
slightly twitching or trembling.
[6] `Mimik und Physiognomik,' 1867, s. 102. Duchenne, Mecanisme de la Phys. Humaine, Album, p. 34.
Infants whilst young do not shed tears or weep, as is well known to nurses and medical men. This
circumstance is not exclusively due to the lacrymal glands being as yet incapable of secreting tears. I first
noticed this fact from having accidentally brushed with the cuff of my coat the open eye of one of my infants,
when seventyseven days old, causing this eye to water freely; and though the child screamed violently, the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 57
Page No 60
other eye remained dry, or was only slightly suffused with tears. A similar slight effusion occurred ten days
previously in both eyes during a screamingfit. The tears did not run over the eyelids and roll down the
cheeks of this child, whilst screaming badly, when 122 days old. This first happened 17 days later, at the age
of 139 days. A few other children have been observed for me, and the period of free weeping appears to be
very variable. In one case, the eyes became slightly suffused at the age of only 20 days; in another, at 62
days. With two other children, the tears did NOT run down the face at the ages of 84 and 110 days; but in a
third child they did run down at the age of 104 days. In one instance, as I was positively assured, tears ran
down at the unusually early age of 42 days. It would appear as if the lacrymal glands required some practice
in the individual before they are easily excited into action, in somewhat the same manner as various inherited
consensual movements and tastes require some exercise before they are fixed and perfected. This is all the
more likely with a habit like weeping, which must have been acquired since the period when man branched
off from the common progenitor of the genus Homo and of the nonweeping anthropomorphous apes.
[7] Dr. Duchenne makes this remark, ibid. p. 39.
The fact of tears not being shed at a very early age from pain or any mental emotion is remarkable, as, later in
life, no expression is more general or more strongly marked than weeping. When the habit has once been
acquired by an infant, it expresses in the clearest manner suffering of all kinds, both bodily pain and mental
distress, even though accompanied by other emotions, such as fear or rage. The character of the crying,
however, changes at a very early age, as I noticed in my own infants,the passionate cry differing from that
of grief. A lady informs me that her child, nine months old, when in a passion screams loudly, but does not
weep; tears, however, are shed when she is punished by her chair being turned with its back to the table. This
difference may perhaps be attributed to weeping being restrained, as we shall immediately see, at a more
advanced age, under most circumstances excepting grief; and to the influence of such restraint being
transmitted to an earlier period of life, than that at which it was first practised.
With adults, especially of the male sex, weeping soon ceases to be caused by, or to express, bodily pain. This
may be accounted for by its being thought weak and unmanly by men, both of civilized and barbarous races,
to exhibit bodily pain by any outward sign. With this exception, savages weep copiously from very slight
causes, of which fact Sir J. Lubbock[8] has collected instances. A New Zealand chief "cried like a child
because the sailors spoilt his favourite cloak by powdering it with flour." I saw in Tierra del Fuego a native
who had lately lost a brother, and who alternately cried with hysterical violence, and laughed heartily at
anything which amused him. With the civilized nations of Europe there is also much difference in the
frequency of weeping. Englishmen rarely cry, except under the pressure of the acutest grief; whereas in some
parts of the Continent the men shed tears much more readily and freely.
The insane notoriously give way to all their emotions with little or no restraint; and I am informed by Dr. J.
Crichton Browne, that nothing is more characteristic of simple melancholia, even in the male sex, than a
tendency to weep on the slightest occasions, or from no cause. They also weep disproportionately on the
occurrence of any real cause of grief. The length of time during which some patients weep is astonishing, as
well as the amount of tears which they shed. One melancholic girl wept for a whole day, and afterwards
confessed to Dr. Browne, that it was because she remembered that she had once shaved off her eyebrows to
promote their growth. Many patients in the asylum sit for a long time rocking themselves backwards and
forwards; "and if spoken to, they stop their movements, purse up their eyes, depress the corners of the mouth,
and burst out crying." In some of these cases, the being spoken to or kindly greeted appears to suggest some
fanciful and sorrowful notion; but in other cases an effort of any kind excites weeping, independently of any
sorrowful idea. Patients suffering from acute mania likewise have paroxysms of violent crying or blubbering,
in the midst of their incoherent ravings. We must not, however, lay too much stress on the copious shedding
of tears by the insane, as being due to the lack of all restraint; for certain braindiseases, as hemiplegia,
brainwasting, and senile decay, have a special tendency to induce weeping. Weeping is common in the
insane, even after a complete state of fatuity has been reached and the power of speech lost. Persons born
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 58
Page No 61
idiotic likewise weep;[9] but it is said that this is not the case with cretins.
[8] `The Origin of Civilization,' 1870, p. 355.
Weeping seems to be the primary and natural expression, as we see in children, of suffering of any kind,
whether bodily pain short of extreme agony, or mental distress. But the foregoing facts and common
experience show us that a frequently repeated effort to restrain weeping, in association with certain states of
the mind, does much in checking the habit. On the other hand, it appears that the power of weeping can be
increased through habit; thus the Rev. R. Taylor,[10] who long resided in New Zealand, asserts that the
women can voluntarily shed tears in abundance; they meet for this purpose to mourn for the dead, and they
take pride in crying "in the most affecting manner."
A single effort of repression brought to bear on the lacrymal glands does little, and indeed seems often to lead
to an opposite result. An old and experienced physician told me that he had always found that the only means
to check the occasional bitter weeping of ladies who consulted him, and who themselves wished to desist,
was earnestly to beg them not to try, and to assure them that nothing would relieve them so much as
prolonged and copious crying.
[9] See, for instance, Mr. Marshall's account of an idiot in Philosoph. Transact. 1864, p. 526. With respect to
cretins, see Dr. Piderit, `Mimik und Physiognomik,' 1867, s. 61.
[10] `New Zealand and its Inhabitants,' 1855, p. 175.
The screaming of infants consists of prolonged expirations, with short and rapid, almost spasmodic
inspirations, followed at a somewhat more advanced age by sobbing. According to Gratiolet,[11] the glottis is
chiefly affected during the act of sobbing. This sound is heard "at the moment when the inspiration conquers
the resistance of the glottis, and the air rushes into the chest." But the whole act of respiration is likewise
spasmodic and violent. The shoulders are at the same time generally raised, as by this movement respiration
is rendered easier. With one of my infants, when seventyseven days old, the inspirations were so rapid and
strong that they approached in character to sobbing; when 138 days old I first noticed distinct sobbing, which
subsequently followed every bad cryingfit. The respiratory movements are partly voluntary and partly
involuntary, and I apprehend that sobbing is at least in part due to children having some power to command
after early infancy their vocal organs and to stop their screams, but from having less power over their
respiratory muscles, these continue for a time to act in an involuntary or spasmodic manner, after having been
brought into violent action. Sobbing seems to be peculiar to the human species; for the keepers in the
Zoological Gardens assure me that they have never heard a sob from any kind of monkey; though monkeys
often scream loudly whilst being chased and caught, and then pant for a long time. We thus see that there is a
close analogy between sobbing and the free shedding of tears; for with children, sobbing does not commence
during early infancy, but afterwards comes on rather suddenly and then follows every bad cryingfit, until the
habit is checked with advancing years.
[11] `De la Physionomie,' 1865, p. 126.
_On the cause of the contraction of the muscles round the eyes during screaming_.We have seen that
infants and young children, whilst screaming, invariably close their eyes firmly, by the contraction of the
surrounding muscles, so that the skin becomes wrinkled all around. With older children, and even with adults,
whenever there is violent and unrestrained crying, a tendency to the contraction of these same muscles may
be observed; though this is often checked in order not to interfere with vision.
Sir C. Bell explains[12] this action in the following manner:"During every violent act of expiration,
whether in hearty laughter, weeping, coughing, or sneezing, the eyeball is firmly compressed by the fibres of
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 59
Page No 62
the orbicularis; and this is a provision for supporting and defending the vascular system of the interior of the
eye from a retrograde impulse communicated to the blood in the veins at that time. When we contract the
chest and expel the air, there is a retardation of the blood in the veins of the neck and head; and in the more
powerful acts of expulsion, the blood not only distends the vessels, but is even regurgitated into the minute
branches. Were the eye not properly compressed at that time, and a resistance given to the shock, irreparable
injury might be inflicted on the delicate textures of the interior of the eye." He further adds, "If we separate
the eyelids of a child to examine the eye, while it cries and struggles with passion, by taking off the natural
support to the vascular system of the eye, and means of guarding it against the rush of blood then occurring,
the conjunctiva becomes suddenly filled with blood, and the eyelids everted."
[12] `The Anatomy of Expression,' 1844, p. 106. See also his paper in the `Philosophical Transactions,' 1822,
p. 284, ibid. 1823, pp. 166 and 289. Also `The Nervous System of the Human Body,' 3rd edit. 1836, p. 175.
Not only are the muscles round the eyes strongly contracted, as Sir C. Bell states and as I have often
observed, during screaming, loud laughter, coughing, and sneezing, but during several other analogous
actions. A man contracts these muscles when he violently blows his nose. I asked one of my boys to shout as
loudly as he possibly could, and as soon as he began, he firmly contracted his orbicular muscles; I observed
this repeatedly, and on asking him why he had every time so firmly closed his eyes, I found that he was quite
unaware of the fact: he had acted instinctively or unconsciously.
It is not necessary, in order to lead to the contraction of these muscles, that air should actually be expelled
from the chest; it suffices that the muscles of the chest and abdomen should contract with great force, whilst
by the closure of the glottis no air escapes. In violent vomiting or retching the diaphragm is made to descend
by the chest being filled with air; it is then held in this position by the closure of the glottis, "as well as by the
contraction of its own fibres."[13] The abdominal muscles now contract strongly upon the stomach, its proper
muscles likewise contracting, and the contents are thus ejected. During each effort of vomiting "the head
becomes greatly congested, so that the features are red and swollen, and the large veins of the face and
temples visibly dilated." At the same time, as I know from observation, the muscles round the eyes are
strongly contracted. This is likewise the case when the abdominal muscles act downwards with unusual force
in expelling the contents of the intestinal canal.
[13] See Dr. Brinton's account of the act of vomiting, in Todd's Cyclop. of Anatomy and Physiology, 1859,
vol. v. Supplement, p. 318.
The greatest exertion of the muscles of the body, if those of the chest are not brought into strong action in
expelling or compressing the air within the lungs, does not lead to the contraction of the muscles round the
eyes. I have observed my sons using great force in gymnastic exercises, as in repeatedly raising their
suspended bodies by their arms alone, and in lifting heavy weights from the ground, but there was hardly any
trace of contraction in the muscles round the eyes.
As the contraction of these muscles for the protection of the eyes during violent expiration is indirectly, as we
shall hereafter see, a fundamental element in several of our most important expressions, I was extremely
anxious to ascertain how far Sir C. Bell's view could be substantiated. Professor Donders, of Utrecht,[14]
well known as one of the highest authorities in Europe on vision and on the structure of the eye, has most
kindly undertaken for me this investigation with the aid of the many ingenious mechanisms of modern
science, and has published the results.[15] He shows that during violent expiration the external, the
intraocular, and the retroocular vessels of the eye are all affected in two ways, namely by the increased
pressure of the blood in the arteries, and by the return of the blood in the veins being impeded. It is, therefore,
certain that both the arteries and the veins of the eye are more or less distended during violent expiration. The
evidence in detail may be found in Professor Donders' valuable memoir. We see the effects on the veins of
the head, in their prominence, and in the purple colour of the face of a man who coughs violently from being
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 60
Page No 63
half choked. I may mention, on the same authority, that the whole eye certainly advances a little during each
violent expiration. This is due to the dilatation of the retroocular vessels, and might have been expected
from the intimate connection of the eye and brain; the brain being known to rise and fall with each
respiration, when a portion of the skull has been removed; and as may be seen along the unclosed sutures of
infants' heads. This also, I presume, is the reason that the eyes of a strangled man appear as if they were
starting from their sockets.
[14] I am greatly indebted to Mr. Bowman for having introduced me to Prof. Donders, and for his aid in
persuading this great physiologist to undertake the investigation of the present subject. I am likewise much
indebted to Mr. Bowman for having given me, with the utmost kindness, information on many points.
[15] This memoir first appeared in the `Nederlandsch Archief voor Genees en Natuurkiinde,' Deel 5, 1870. It
has been translated by Dr. W. D. Moore, under the title of "On the Action of the Eyelids in determination of
Blood from expiratory effort," in `Archives of Medicine,' edited by Dr. L. S. Beale, 1870, vol. v. p. 20.
With respect to the protection of the eye during violent expiratory efforts by the pressure of the eyelids,
Professor Donders concludes from his various observations that this action certainly limits or entirely
removes the dilatation of the vessels.[16] At such times, he adds, we not unfrequently see the hand
involuntarily laid upon the eyelids, as if the better to support and defend the eyeball.
[16] Prof. Donders remarks (ibid. p. 28), that, "After injury to the eye, after operations, and in some forms of
internal inflammation, we attach great value to the uniform support of the closed eyelids, and we increase this
in many instances by the application of a bandage. In both cases we carefully endeavour to avoid great
expiratory pressure, the disadvantage of which is well known." Mr. Bowman informs me that in the excessive
photophobia, accompanying what is called scrofulous ophthalmia in children, when the light is so very
painful that during weeks or months it is constantly excluded by the most forcible closure of the lids, he has
often been struck on opening the lids by the paleness of the eye, not an unnatural paleness, but an absence
of the redness that might have been expected when the surface is somewhat inflamed, as is then usually the
case; and this paleness he is inclined to attribute to the forcible closure of the eyelids.
Nevertheless much evidence cannot at present be advanced to prove that the eye actually suffers injury from
the want of support during violent expiration; but there is some. It is "a fact that forcible expiratory efforts in
violent coughing or vomiting, and especially in sneezing, sometimes give rise to ruptures of the little
(external) vessels" of the eye.[17] With respect to the internal vessels, Dr. Gunning has lately recorded a case
of exophthalmos in consequence of whoopingcough, which in his opinion depended on the rupture of the
deeper vessels; and another analogous case has been recorded. But a mere sense of discomfort would
probably suffice to lead to the associated habit of protecting the eyeball by the contraction of the surrounding
muscles. Even the expectation or chance of injury would probably be sufficient, in the same manner as an
object moving too near the eye induces involuntary winking of the eyelids. We may, therefore, safely
conclude from Sir C. Bell's observations, and more especially from the more careful investigations by
Professor Donders, that the firm closure of the eyelids during the screaming of children is an action full of
meaning and of real service.
We have already seen that the contraction of the orbicular muscles leads to the drawing up of the upper lip,
and consequently, if the mouth is kept widely open, to the drawing down of the corners by the contraction of
the depressor muscles. The formation of the nasolabial fold on the cheeks likewise follows from the
drawing up of the upper lip. Thus all the chief expressive movements of the face during crying apparently
result from the contraction of the muscles round the eyes. We shall also find that the shedding of tears
depends on, or at least stands in some connection with, the contraction of these same muscles.
[17] Donders, ibid. p. 36.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 61
Page No 64
In some of the foregoing cases, especially in those of sneezing and coughing, it is possible that the
contraction of the orbicular muscles may serve in addition to protect the eyes from too severe a jar or
vibration. I think so, because dogs and cats, in crunching hard bones, always close their eyelids, and at least
sometimes in sneezing; though dogs do not do so whilst barking loudly. Mr. Sutton carefully observed for me
a young orang and chimpanzee, and he found that both always closed their eyes in sneezing and coughing,
but not whilst screaming violently. I gave a small pinch of snuff to a monkey of the American division,
namely, a Cebus, and it closed its eyelids whilst sneezing; but not on a subsequent occasion whilst uttering
loud cries.
_Cause of the secretion of tears_.It is an important fact which must be considered in any theory of the
secretion of tears from the mind being affected, that whenever the muscles round the eyes are strongly and
involuntarily contracted in order to compress the bloodvessels and thus to protect the eyes, tears are
secreted, often in sufficient abundance to roll down the cheeks. This occurs under the most opposite
emotions, and under no emotion at all. The sole exception, and this is only a partial one, to the existence of a
relation between the involuntary and strong contraction of these muscles and the secretion of tears is that of
young infants, who, whilst screaming violently with their eyelids firmly closed, do not commonly weep until
they have attained the age of from two to three or four months. Their eyes, however, become suffused with
tears at a much earlier age. It would appear, as already remarked, that the lacrymal glands do not, from the
want of practice or some other cause, come to full functional activity at a very early period of life. With
children at a somewhat later age, crying out or wailing from any distress is so regularly accompanied by the
shedding of tears, that weeping and crying are synonymous terms.[18]
Under the opposite emotion of great joy or amusement, as long as laughter is moderate there is hardly any
contraction of the muscles round the eyes, so that there is no frowning; but when peals of loud laughter are
uttered, with rapid and violent spasmodic expirations, tears stream down the face. I have more than once
noticed the face of a person, after a paroxysm of violent laughter, and I could see that the orbicular muscles
and those running to the upper lip were still partially contracted, which together with the tearstained cheeks
gave to the upper half of the face an expression not to be distinguished from that of a child still blubbering
from grief. The fact of tears streaming down the face during violent laughter is common to all the races of
mankind, as we shall see in a future chapter.
In violent coughing especially when a person is halfchoked, the face becomes purple, the veins distended,
the orbicular muscles strongly contracted, and tears run down the cheeks. Even after a fit of ordinary
coughing, almost every one has to wipe his eyes. In violent vomiting or retching, as I have myself
experienced and seen in others, the orbicular muscles are strongly contracted, and tears sometimes flow freely
down the cheeks. It has been suggested to me that this may be due to irritating matter being injected into the
nostrils, and causing by reflex action the secretion of tears. Accordingly I asked one of my informants, a
surgeon, to attend to the effects of retching when nothing was thrown up from the stomach; and, by an odd
coincidence, he himself suffered the next morning from an attack of retching, and three days subsequently
observed a lady under a similar attack; and he is certain that in neither case an atom of matter was ejected
from the stomach; yet the orbicular muscles were strongly contracted, and tears freely secreted. I can also
speak positively to the energetic contraction of these same muscles round the eyes, and to the coincident free
secretion of tears, when the abdominal muscles act with unusual force in a downward direction on the
intestinal canal.
[18] Mr. Hensleigh Wedgwood (Dict. of English Etymology, 1859, vol. i. p. 410) says, "the verb to weep
comes from AngloSaxon _wop_, the primary meaning of which is simply outcry."
Yawning commences with a deep inspiration, followed by a long and forcible expiration; and at the same
time almost all the muscles of the body are strongly contracted, including those round the eyes. During this
act tears are often secreted, and I have seen them even rolling down the cheeks.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 62
Page No 65
I have frequently observed that when persons scratch some point which itches intolerably, they forcibly close
their eyelids; but they do not, as I believe, first draw a deep breath and then expel it with force; and I have
never noticed that the eyes then become filled with tears; but I am not prepared to assert that this does not
occur. The forcible closure of the eyelids is, perhaps, merely a part of that general action by which almost all
the muscles of the body are at the same time rendered rigid. It is quite different from the gentle closure of the
eyes which often accompanies, as Gratiolet remarks,[19] the smelling a delicious odour, or the tasting a
delicious morsel, and which probably originates in the desire to shut out any disturbing impression through
the eyes.
[19] `De la Physionomie,' 1865, p. 217.
Professor Donders writes to me to the following effect: "I have observed some cases of a very curious
affection when, after a slight rub (_attouchement_), for example, from the friction of a coat, which caused
neither a wound nor a contusion, spasms of the orbicular muscles occurred, with a very profuse flow of tears,
lasting about one hour. Subsequently, sometimes after an interval of several weeks, violent spasms of the
same muscles reoccurred, accompanied by the secretion of tears, together with primary or secondary
redness of the eye." Mr. Bowman informs me that be has occasionally observed closely analogous cases, and
that, in some of these, there was no redness or inflammation of the eyes.
I was anxious to ascertain whether there existed in any of the lower animals a similar relation between the
contraction of the orbicular muscles during violent expiration and the secretion of tears; but there are very
few animals which contract these muscles in a prolonged manner, or which shed tears. _The Macacus
maurus_, which formerly wept so copiously in the Zoological Gardens, would have been a fine case for
observation; but the two monkeys now there, and which are believed to belong to the same species, do not
weep. Nevertheless they were carefully observed by Mr. Bartlett and myself, whilst screaming loudly, and
they seemed to contract these muscles; but they moved about their cages so rapidly, that it was difficult to
observe with certainty. No other monkey, as far as I have been able to ascertain, contracts its orbicular
muscles whilst screaming.
The Indian elephant is known sometimes to weep. Sir E. Tennent, in describing these which he saw captured
and bound in Ceylon, says, some "lay motionless on the ground, with no other indication of suffering than the
tears which suffused their eyes and flowed incessantly." Speaking of another elephant he says, "When
overpowered and made fast, his grief was most affecting; his violence sank to utter prostration, and he lay on
the ground, uttering choking cries, with tears trickling down his cheeks."[20] In the Zoological Gardens the
keeper of the Indian elephants positively asserts that he has several times seen tears rolling down the face of
the old female, when distressed by the removal of the young one. Hence I was extremely anxious to ascertain,
as an extension of the relation between the contraction of the orbicular muscles and the shedding of tears in
man, whether elephants when screaming or trumpeting loudly contract these muscles. At Mr. Bartlett's desire
the keeper ordered the old and the young elephant to trumpet; and we repeatedly saw in both animals that,
just as the trumpeting began, the orbicular muscles, especially the lower ones, were distinctly contracted. On
a subsequent occasion the keeper made the old elephant trumpet much more loudly, and invariably both the
upper and lower orbicular muscles were strongly contracted, and now in an equal degree. It is a singular fact
that the African elephant, which, however, is so different from the Indian species that it is placed by some
naturalists in a distinct subgenus, when made on two occasions to trumpet loudly, exhibited no trace of the
contraction of the orbicular muscles.
[20] `Ceylon,' 3rd edit. 1859, vol. ii. pp. 364, 376. I applied to Mr. Thwaites, in Ceylon, for further
information with respect to the weeping of the elephant; and in consequence received a letter from the Rev.
Mr Glenie, who, with others, kindly observed for me a herd of recently captured elephants. These, when
irritated, screamed violently; but it is remarkable that they never when thus screaming contracted the muscles
round the eyes. Nor did they shed tears; and the native hunters asserted that they had never observed
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 63
Page No 66
elephants weeping. Nevertheless, it appears to me impossible to doubt Sir E. Tennent's distinct details about
their weeping, supported as they are by the positive assertion of the keeper in the Zoological Gardens. It is
certain that the two elephants in the Gardens, when they began to trumpet loudly, invariably contracted their
orbicular muscles. I can reconcile these conflicting statements only by supposing that the recently captured
elephants in Ceylon, from being enraged or frightened, desired to observe their persecutors, and consequently
did not contract their orbicular muscles, so that their vision might not be impeded. Those seen weeping by Sir
E. Tennent were prostrate, and had given up the contest in despair. The elephants which trumpeted in the
Zoological Gardens at the word of command, were, of course, neither alarmed nor enraged.
From the several foregoing cases with respect to Man, there can, I think, be no doubt that the contraction of
the muscles round the eyes, during violent expiration or when the expanded chest is forcibly compressed, is,
in some manner, intimately connected with the secretion of tears. This holds good under widely different
emotions, and independently of any emotion. It is not, of course, meant that tears cannot be secreted without
the contraction of these muscles; for it is notorious that they are often freely shed with the eyelids not closed,
and with the brows unwrinkled. The contraction must be both involuntary and prolonged, as during a choking
fit, or energetic, as during a sneeze. The mere involuntary winking of the eyelids, though often repeated, does
not bring tears into the eyes. Nor does the voluntary and prolonged contraction of the several surrounding
muscles suffice. As the lacrymal glands of children are easily excited, I persuaded my own and several other
children of different ages to contract these muscles repeatedly with their utmost force, and to continue doing
so as long as they possibly could; but this produced hardly any effect. There was sometimes a little moisture
in the eyes, but not more than apparently could be accounted for by the squeezing out of the already secreted
tears within the glands.
The nature of the relation between the involuntary and energetic contraction of the muscles round the eyes,
and the secretion of tears, cannot be positively ascertained, but a probable view may be suggested. The
primary function of the secretion of tears, together with some mucus, is to lubricate the surface of the eye;
and a secondary one, as some believe, is to keep the nostrils damp, so that the inhaled air may be moist,[21]
and likewise to favour the power of smelling. But another, and at least equally important function of tears, is
to wash out particles of dust or other minute objects which may get into the eyes. That this is of great
importance is clear from the cases in which the cornea has been rendered opaque through inflammation,
caused by particles of dust not being removed, in consequence of the eye and eyelid becoming
immovable.[22] The secretion of tears from the irritation of any foreign body in the eye is a reflex
action;that is, the body irritates a peripheral nerve which sends an impression to certain sensory
nervecells; these transmit an influence to other cells, and these again to the lacrymal glands. The influence
transmitted to these glands causes, as there is good reason to believe, the relaxation of the muscular coats of
the smaller arteries; this allows more blood to permeate the glandular tissue, and this induces a free secretion
of tears. When the small arteries of the face, including those of the retina, are relaxed under very different
circumstances, namely, during an intense blush, the lacrymal glands are sometimes affected in a like manner,
for the eyes become suffused with tears.
It is difficult to conjecture how many reflex actions have originated, but, in relation to the present case of the
affection of the lacrymal glands through irritation of the surface of the eye, it may be worth remarking that, as
soon as some primordial form became semiterrestrial in its habits, and was liable to get particles of dust into
its eyes, if these were not washed out they would cause much irritation; and on the principle of the radiation
of nerveforce to adjoining nervecells, the lacrymal glands would be stimulated to secretion. As this would
often recur, and as nerveforce readily passes along accustomed channels, a slight irritation would ultimately
suffice to cause a free secretion of tears.
[21] Bergeon, as quoted in the `Journal of Anatomy and Physiology,' Nov. 1871, p. 235.
[22] See, for instance, a case given by Sir Charles Bell, `Philosophical Transactions,' 1823, p. 177.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 64
Page No 67
As soon as by this, or by some other means, a reflex action of this nature had been established and rendered
easy, other stimulants applied to the surface of the eyesuch as a cold wind, slow inflammatory action, or a
blow on the eyelids would cause a copious secretion of tears, as we know to be the case. The glands are
also excited into action through the irritation of adjoining parts. Thus when the nostrils are irritated by
pungent vapours, though the eyelids may be kept firmly closed, tears are copiously secreted; and this likewise
follows from a blow on the nose, for instance from a boxingglove. A stinging switch on the face produces,
as I have seen, the same effect. In these latter cases the secretion of tears is an incidental result, and of no
direct service. As all these parts of the face, including the lacrymal glands, are supplied with branches of the
same nerve, namely, the fifth, it is in some degree intelligible that the effects of the excitement of any one
branch should spread to the nervecells or roots of the other branches.
The internal parts of the eye likewise act, under certain conditions, in a reflex manner on the lacrymal glands.
The following statements have been kindly communicated to me by Mr. Bowman; but the subject is a very
intricate one, as all the parts of the eye are so intimately related together, and are so sensitive to various
stimulants. A strong light acting on the retina, when in a normal condition, has very little tendency to cause
lacrymation; but with unhealthy children having small, oldstanding ulcers on the cornea, the retina becomes
excessively sensitive to light, and exposure even to common daylight causes forcible and sustained closure of
the lids, and a profuse flow of tears. When persons who ought to begin the use of convex glasses habitually
strain the waning power of accommodation, an undue secretion of tears very often follows, and the retina is
liable to become unduly sensitive to light. In general, morbid affections of the surface of the eye, and of the
ciliary structures concerned in the accommodative act, are prone to be accompanied with excessive secretion
of tears. Hardness of the eyeball, not rising to inflammation, but implying a want of balance between the
fluids poured out and again taken up by the intraocular vessels, is not usually attended with any
lacrymation. When the balance is on the other side, and the eye becomes too soft, there is a greater tendency
to lacrymation. Finally, there are numerous morbid states and structural alterations of the eyes, and even
terrible inflammations, which may be attended with little or no secretion of tears.
It also deserves notice, as indirectly bearing on our subject, that the eye and adjoining parts are subject to an
extraordinary number of reflex and associated movements, sensations, and actions, besides those relating to
the lacrymal glands. When a bright light strikes the retina of one eye alone, the iris contracts, but the iris of
the other eye moves after a measurable interval of time. The iris likewise moves in accommodation to near or
distant vision, and when the two eyes are made to converge.[23] Every one knows how irresistibly the
eyebrows are drawn down under an intensely bright light. The eyelids also involuntarily wink when an object
is moved near the eyes, or a sound is suddenly heard. The wellknown case of a bright light causing some
persons to sneeze is even more curious; for nerveforce here radiates from certain nervecells in connection
with the retina, to the sensory nervecells of the nose, causing it to tickle; and from these, to the cells which
command the various respiratory muscles (the orbiculars included) which expel the air in so peculiar a
manner that it rushes through the nostrils alone.
To return to our point: why are tears secreted during a screamingfit or other violent expiratory efforts? As a
slight blow on the eyelids causes a copious secretion of tears, it is at least possible that the spasmodic
contraction of the eyelids, by pressing strongly on the eyeball, should in a similar manner cause some
secretion. This seems possible, although the voluntary contraction of the same muscles does not produce any
such effect. We know that a man cannot voluntarily sneeze or cough with nearly the same force as he does
automatically; and so it is with the contraction of the orbicular muscles: Sir C. Bell experimented on them,
and found that by suddenly and forcibly closing the eyelids in the dark, sparks of light are seen, like those
caused by tapping the eyelids with the fingers; "but in sneezing the compression is both more rapid and more
forcible, and the sparks are more brilliant." That these sparks are due to the contraction of the eyelids is clear,
because if they "are held open during the act of sneezing, no sensation of light will be experienced." In the
peculiar cases referred to by Professor Donders and Mr. Bowman, we have seen that some weeks after the
eye has been very slightly injured, spasmodic contractions of the eyelids ensue, and these are accompanied by
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 65
Page No 68
a profuse flow of tears. In the act of yawning, the tears are apparently due solely to the spasmodic contraction
of the muscles round the eyes. Notwithstanding these latter cases, it seems hardly credible that the pressure of
the eyelids on the surface of the eye, although effected spasmodically and therefore with much greater force
than can be done voluntarily, should be sufficient to cause by reflex action the secretion of tears in the many
cases in which this occurs during violent expiratory efforts.
[23] See, on these several points, Prof. Donders `On the Anomalies of Accommodation and Refraction of the
Eye,' 1864, p. 573.
Another cause may come conjointly into play. We have seen that the internal parts of the eye, under certain
conditions act in a reflex manner on the lacrymal glands. We know that during violent expiratory efforts the
pressure of the arterial blood within the vessels of the eye is increased, and that the return of the venous blood
is impeded. It seems, therefore, not improbable that the distension of the ocular vessels, thus induced, might
act by reflection on the lacrymal glandsthe effects due to the spasmodic pressure of the eyelids on the
surface of the eye being thus increased.
In considering how far this view is probable, we should bear in mind that the eyes of infants have been acted
on in this double manner during numberless generations, whenever they have screamed; and on the principle
of nerveforce readily passing along accustomed channels, even a moderate compression of the eyeballs and
a moderate distension of the ocular vessels would ultimately come, through habit, to act on the glands. We
have an analogous case in the orbicular muscles being almost always contracted in some slight degree, even
during a gentle cryingfit, when there can be no distension of the vessels and no uncomfortable sensation
excited within the eyes.
Moreover, when complex actions or movements have long been performed in strict association together, and
these are from any cause at first voluntarily and afterwards habitually checked, then if the proper exciting
conditions occur, any part of the action or movement which is least under the control of the will, will often
still be involuntarily performed. The secretion by a gland is remarkably free from the influence of the will;
therefore, when with the advancing age of the individual, or with the advancing culture of the race, the habit
of crying out or screaming is restrained, and there is consequently no distension of the bloodvessels of the
eye, it may nevertheless well happen that tears should still be secreted. We may see, as lately remarked, the
muscles round the eyes of a person who reads a pathetic story, twitching or trembling in so slight a degree as
hardly to be detected. In this case there has been no screaming and no distension of the bloodvessels, yet
through habit certain nervecells send a small amount of nerveforce to the cells commanding the muscles
round the eyes; and they likewise send some to the cells commanding the lacrymal glands, for the eyes often
become at the same time just moistened with tears. If the twitching of the muscles round the eyes and the
secretion of tears had been completely prevented, nevertheless it is almost certain that there would have been
some tendency to transmit nerveforce in these same directions; and as the lacrymal glands are remarkably
free from the control of the will, they would be eminently liable still to act, thus betraying, though there were
no other outward signs, the pathetic thoughts which were passing through the person's mind.
As a further illustration of the view here advanced, I may remark that if, during an early period of life, when
habits of all kinds are readily established, our infants, when pleased, had been accustomed to utter loud peals
of laughter (during which the vessels of their eyes are distended) as often and as continuously as they have
yielded when distressed to screamingfits, then it is probable that in after life tears would have been as
copiously and as regularly secreted under the one state of mind as under the other. Gentle laughter, or a smile,
or even a pleasing thought, would have sufficed to cause a moderate secretion of tears. There does indeed
exist an evident tendency in this direction, as will be seen in a future chapter, when we treat of the tender
feelings. With the Sandwich Islanders, according to Freycinet,[24] tears are actually recognized as a sign of
happiness; but we should require better evidence on this head than that of a passing voyager. So again if our
infants, during many generations, and each of them during several years, had almost daily suffered from
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING. 66
Page No 69
prolonged chokingfits, during which the vessels of the eye are distended and tears copiously secreted, then it
is probable, such is the force of associated habit, that during after life the mere thought of a choke, without
any distress of mind, would have sufficed to bring tears into our eyes.
To sum up this chapter, weeping is probably the result of some such chain of events as follows. Children,
when wanting food or suffering in any way, cry out loudly, like the young of most other animals, partly as a
call to their parents for aid, and partly from any great exertion serving relief. Prolonged screaming inevitably
leads to the gorging of the bloodvessels of the eye; and this will have led, at first consciously and at last
habitually, to the contraction of the muscles round the eyes in order to protect them. At the same time the
spasmodic pressure on the surface of the eye, and the distension of the vessels within the eye, without
necessarily entailing any conscious sensation, will have affected, through reflex action, the lacrymal glands.
Finally, through the three principles of nerveforce readily passing along accustomed channelsof
association, which is so widely extended in its powerand of certain actions, being more under the control
of the will than othersit has come to pass that suffering readily causes the secretion of tears, without being
necessarily accompanied by any other action.
[24] Quoted by Sir J. Lubbock, `Prehistoric Times,' 1865, p. 458.
Although in accordance with this view we must look at weeping as an incidental result, as purposeless as the
secretion of tears from a blow outside the eye, or as a sneeze from the retina being affected by a bright light,
yet this does not present any difficulty in our understanding how the secretion of tears serves as a relief to
suffering. And by as much as the weeping is more violent or hysterical, by so much will the relief be
greater, on the same principle that the writhing of the whole body, the grinding of the teeth, and the
uttering of piercing shrieks, all give relief under an agony of pain.
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR.
General effect of grief on the systemObliquity of the eyebrows under sufferingOn the cause of the
obliquity of the eyebrows On the depression of the corners of the mouth.
AFTER the mind has suffered from an acute paroxysm of grief, and the cause still continues, we fall into a
state of low spirits; or we may be utterly cast down and dejected. Prolonged bodily pain, if not amounting to
an agony, generally leads to the same state of mind. If we expect to suffer, we are anxious; if we have no
hope of relief, we despair.
Persons suffering from excessive grief often seek relief by violent and almost frantic movements, as
described in a former chapter; but when their suffering is somewhat mitigated, yet prolonged, they no longer
wish for action, but remain motionless and passive, or may occasionally rock themselves to and fro. The
circulation becomes languid; the face pale; the muscles flaccid; the eyelids droop; the head hangs on the
contracted chest; the lips, checks, and lower jaw all sink downwards from their own weight. Hence all the
features are lengthened; and the face of a person who hears bad news is said to fall. A party of natives in
Tierra del Fuego endeavoured to explain to us that their friend, the captain of a sealing vessel, was out of
spirits, by pulling down their cheeks with both hands, so as to make their faces as long as possible. Mr.
Bunnet informs me that the Australian aborigines when out of spirits have a chopfallen appearance. After
prolonged suffering the eyes become dull and lack expression, and are often slightly suffused with tears. The
eyebrows not rarely are rendered oblique, which is due to their inner ends being raised. This produces
peculiarlyformed wrinkles on the forehead, which are very different from those of a simple frown; though in
some cases a frown alone may be present. The corners of the mouth are drawn downwards, which is so
universally recognized as a sign of being out of spirits, that it is almost proverbial.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 67
Page No 70
The breathing becomes slow and feeble, and is often interrupted by deep sighs. As Gratiolet remarks,
whenever our attention is long concentrated on any subject, we forget to breathe, and then relieve ourselves
by a deep inspiration; but the sighs of a sorrowful person, owing to his slow respiration and languid
circulation, are eminently characteristic.[1] As the grief of a person in this state occasionally recurs and
increases into a paroxysm, spasms affect the respiratory muscles, and he feels as if something, the socalled
_globus hystericus_, was rising in his throat. These spasmodic movements are clearly allied to the sobbing of
children, and are remnants of those severer spasms which occur when a person is said to choke from
excessive grief.[2]
[1] The above descriptive remarks are taken in part from my own observations, but chiefly from Gratiolet
(`De la Physionomie,' pp. 53, 337; on Sighing, 232), who has well treated this whole subject. See, also,
Huschke. `Mimices et Physiognomices, Fragmentum Physiologicitim,' 1821, p. 21. On the dulness of the
eyes, Dr. Piderit, `Mimik und Physiognomik,' 1867, s. 65.
[2] On the action of grief on the organs of respiration,
_Obliquity of the eyebrows_.Two points alone in the above description require further elucidation, and
these are very curious ones; namely, the raising of the inner ends of the eyebrows, and the drawing down of
the corners of the mouth. With respect to the eyebrows, they may occasionally be seen to assume an oblique
position in persons suffering from deep dejection or anxiety; for instance, I have observed this movement in a
mother whilst speaking about her sick son; and it is sometimes excited by quite trifling or momentary causes
of real or pretended distress. The eyebrows assume this position owing to the contraction of certain muscles
(namely, the orbiculars, corrugators, and pyramidals of the nose, which together tend to lower and contract
the eyebrows) being partially checked by the more powerful action of the central fascim of the frontal
muscle. These latter fasciae by their contraction raise the inner ends alone of the eyebrows; and as the
corrugators at the same time draw the eyebrows together, their inner ends become puckered into a fold or
lump. This fold is a highly characteristic point in the appearance of the eyebrows when rendered oblique, as
may be seen in figs. 2 and 5, Plate II. The eyebrows are at the same time somewhat roughened, owing to the
hairs being made to project. Dr. J. Crichton Browne has also often noticed in melancholic patients who keep
their eyebrows persistently oblique, "a peculiar acute arching of the upper eyelid." A trace of this may be
observed by comparing the right and left eyelids of the young man in the photograph (fig. 2, Plate II.); for he
was not able to act equally on both eyebrows. This is also shown by the unequal furrows on the two sides of
his forehead. The acute arching of the eyelids
see more especially Sir C. Bell, `Anatomy of Expression,' 3rd edit. 1844, p. 151. depends, I believe, on the
inner end alone of the eyebrows being raised; for when the whole eyebrow is elevated and arched, the upper
eyelid follows in a slight degree the same movement.
But the most conspicuous result of the opposed contraction of the abovenamed muscles, is exhibited by the
peculiar furrows formed on the forehead. These muscles, when thus in conjoint yet opposed action, may be
called, for the sake of brevity, the griefmuscles. When a person elevates his eyebrows by the contraction of
the whole frontal muscle, transverse wrinkles extend across the whole breadth of the forehead; but in the
present case the middle fasciae alone are contracted; consequently, transverse furrows are formed across the
middle part alone of the forehead. The skin over the exterior parts of both eyebrows is at the same time drawn
downwards and smooth, by the contraction of the outer portions of the orbicular muscles. The eyebrows are
likewise brought together through the simultaneous contraction of the corrugators;[3] and this latter action
generates vertical furrows, separating the exterior and lowered part of the skin of the forehead from the
central and raised part. The union of these vertical furrows with the central and transverse furrows (see figs. 2
and 3) produces a mark on the forehead which has been compared to a horseshoe; but the furrows more
strictly form three sides of a quadrangle. They are often conspicuous on the foreheads of adult or nearly adult
persons, when their eyebrows are made oblique; but with young children, owing to their skin not easily
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 68
Page No 71
wrinkling, they are rarely seen, or mere traces of them can be detected.
These peculiar furrows are best represented in fig. 3, Plate II., on the forehead of a young lady who has the
power in an unusual degree of voluntarily acting on the requisite muscles. As she was absorbed in the
attempt, whilst being photographed, her expression was not at all one of grief; I have therefore given the
forehead alone. Fig. 1 on the same plate, copied from Dr. Duchenne's work 4 represents, on a reduced scale,
the face, in its natural state, of a young man who was a good actor. In fig. 2 he is shown simulating grief, but
the
[3] In the foregoing remarks on the manner in which the eyebrows are made oblique, I have followed what
seems to be the universal opinion of all the anatomists, whose works I have consulted on the action of the
abovenamed muscles, or with whom I have conversed. Hence throughout this work I shall take a similar
view of the action of the _corrugator supercilii_, _orbicularis, pyramidalis nasi_, and _frontalis_ muscles. Dr.
Duchenne, however, believes, and every conclusion at which he arrives deserves serious consideration, that it
is the corrugator, called by him the _sourcilier_, which raises the inner corner of the eyebrows and is
antagonistic to the upper and inner part of the orbicular muscle, as well as to the _pyramidalis nasi_ (see
Mecanisme de la Phys. Humaine, 1862, folio, art. v., text and figures 19 to 29: octavo edit. 1862, p. 43 text).
He admits, however, that the corrugator draws together the eyebrows, causing vertical furrows above the base
of the nose, or a frown. He further believes that towards the outer twothirds of the eyebrow the corrugator
acts in conjunction with the upper orbicular muscle; both here standing in antagonism to the frontal muscle. I
am unable to understand, judging from Henle's drawings (woodcut, fig. 3), how the corrugator can act in the
manner described by Duchenue. See, also, oil this subject, Prof. Donders' remarks in the `Archives of
Medicine,' 1870, vol. v. p. 34. Mr. J. Wood, who is so well known for his careful study of the muscles of the
human frame, informs me that he believes the account which I have given of the action of the corrugator to be
correct. But this is not a point of any importance with respect to the expression which is caused by the
obliquity of the eyebrows, nor of much importance to the theory of its origin.
`I am greatly indebted to Dr. Duchenne for permission to have these two photographs (figs. 1 and 2)
reproduced by the heliotype process from his work in folio. Many of the foregoing remarks on the furrowing
of the skin, when the eyebrows are rendered oblique, are taken from his excellent discussion on this subject.
two eyebrows, as before remarked, are not equally acted on. That the expression is true, may be inferred from
the fact that out of fifteen persons, to whom the original photograph was shown, without any clue to what was
intended being given them, fourteen immediately answered, "despairing sorrow," "suffering endurance,"
"melancholy," and so forth. The history of fig. 5 is rather curious: I saw the photograph in a shopwindow,
and took it to Mr. Rejlander for the sake of finding out by whom it had been made; remarking to him how
pathetic the expression was. He answered, "I made it, and it was likely to be pathetic, for the boy in a few
minutes burst out crying." He then showed me a photograph of the same boy in a placid state, which I have
had (fig. 4) reproduced. In fig. 6, a trace of obliquity in the eyebrows may be detected; but this figure, as well
as fig. 7, is given to show the depression of the corners of the mouth, to which subject I shall presently refer.
Few persons, without some practice, can voluntarily act on their griefmuscles; but after repeated trials a
considerable number succeed, whilst others never can. The degree of obliquity in the eyebrows, whether
assumed voluntarily or unconsciously, differs much in different persons. With some who apparently have
unusually strong pyramidal muscles, the contraction of the central fasciae of the frontal muscle, although it
may be energetic, as shown by the quadrangular furrows on the forehead, does not raise the inner ends of the
eyebrows, but only prevents their being so much lowered as they otherwise would have been. As far as I have
been able to observe, the griefmuscles are brought into action much more frequently by children and women
than by men. They are rarely acted on, at least with grownup persons, from bodily pain, but almost
exclusively from mental distress. Two persons who, after some practice, succeeded in acting on their
griefmuscles, found by looking at a mirror that when they made their eyebrows oblique, they
unintentionally at the same time depressed the corners of their mouths; and this is often the case when the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 69
Page No 72
expression is naturally assumed.
The power to bring the griefmuscles freely into play appears to be hereditary, like almost every other human
faculty. A lady belonging to a family famous for having produced an extraordinary number of great actors
and actresses, and who can herself give this expression "with singular precision," told Dr. Crichton Browne
that all her family had possessed the power in a remarkable degree. The same hereditary tendency is said to
have extended, as I likewise hear from Dr. Browne, to the last descendant of the family, which gave rise to
Sir Walter Scott's novel of `Red Gauntlet;' but the hero is described as contracting his forehead into a
horseshoe mark from any strong emotion. I have also seen a young woman whose forehead seemed almost
habitually thus contracted, independently of any emotion being at the time felt.
The griefmuscles are not very frequently brought into play; and as the action is often momentary, it easily
escapes observation. Although the expression, when observed, is universally and instantly recognized as that
of grief or anxiety, yet not one person out of a thousand who has never studied the subject, is able to say
precisely what change passes over the sufferer's face. Hence probably it is that this expression is not even
alluded to, as far as I have noticed, in any work of fiction, with the exception of `Red Gauntlet' and of one
other novel; and the authoress of the latter, as I am informed, belongs to the famous family of actors just
alluded to; so that her attention may have been specially called to the subject.
The ancient Greek sculptors were familiar with the expression, as shown in the statues of the Laocoon and
Arretino; but, as Duchenne remarks, they carried the transverse furrows across the whole breadth of the
forehead, and thus committed a great anatomical mistake: this is likewise the case in some modern statues. It
is, however, more probable that these wonderfully accurate observers intentionally sacrificed truth for the
sake of beauty, than that they made a mistake; for rectangular furrows on the forehead would not have had a
grand appearance on the marble. The expression, in its fully developed condition, is, as far as I can discover,
not often represented in pictures by the old masters, no doubt owing to the same cause; but a lady who is
perfectly familiar with this expression, informs me that in Fra Angelico's `Descent from the Cross,' in
Florence, it is clearly exhibited in one of the figures on the righthand; and I could add a few other instances.
Dr. Crichton Browne, at my request, closely attended to this expression in the numerous insane patients under
his care in the West Riding Asylum; and he is familiar with Duchenne's photographs of the action of the
griefmuscles. He informs me that they may constantly be seen in energetic action in cases of melancholia,
and especially of hypochondria; and that the persistent lines or furrows, due to their habitual contraction, are
characteristic of the physiognomy of the insane belonging to these two classes. Dr. Browne carefully
observed for me during a considerable period three cases of hypochondria, in which the griefmuscles were
persistently contracted. In one of these, a widow, aged 51, fancied that she had lost all her viscera, and that
her whole body was empty. She wore an expression of great distress, and beat her semiclosed hands
rhythmically together for hours. The griefmuscles were permanently contracted, and the upper eyelids
arched. This condition lasted for months; she then recovered, and her countenance resumed its natural
expression. A second case presented nearly the same peculiarities, with the addition that the corners of the
mouth were depressed.
Mr. Patrick Nicol has also kindly observed for me several cases in the Sussex Lunatic Asylum, and has
communicated to me full details with respect to three of them; but they need not here be given. From his
observations on melancholic patients, Mr. Nicol concludes that the inner ends of the eyebrows are almost
always more or less raised, with the wrinkles on the forehead more or less plainly marked. In the case of one
young woman, these wrinkles were observed to be in constant slight play or movement. In some cases the
corners of the mouth are depressed, but often only in a slight degree. Some amount of difference in the
expression of the several melancholic patients could almost always be observed. The eyelids generally droop;
and the skin near their outer corners and beneath them is wrinkled. The nasolabial fold, which runs from the
wings of the nostrils to the corners of the mouth, and which is so conspicuous in blubbering children, is often
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 70
Page No 73
plainly marked in these patients.
Although with the insane the griefmuscles often act persistently; yet in ordinary cases they are sometimes
brought unconsciously into momentary action by ludicrously slight causes. A gentleman rewarded a young
lady by an absurdly small present; she pretended to be offended, and as she upbraided him, her eyebrows
became extremely oblique, with the forehead properly wrinkled. Another young lady and a youth, both in the
highest spirits, were eagerly talking together with extraordinary rapidity; and I noticed that, as often as the
young lady was beaten, and could not get out her words fast enough, her eyebrows went obliquely upwards,
and rectangular furrows were formed on her forehead. She thus each time hoisted a flag of distress; and this
she did halfadozen times in the course of a few minutes. I made no remark on the subject, but on a
subsequent occasion I asked her to act on her griefmuscles; another girl who was present, and who could do
so voluntarily, showing her what was intended. She tried repeatedly, but utterly failed; yet so slight a cause of
distress as not being able to talk quickly enough, sufficed to bring these muscles over and over again into
energetic action.
The expression of grief, due to the contraction of the griefmuscles, is by no means confined to Europeans,
but appears to be common to all the races of mankind. I have, at least, received trustworthy accounts in
regard to Hindoos, Dhangars (one of the aboriginal hilltribes of India, and therefore belonging to a quite
distinct race from the Hindoos), Malays, Negroes and Australians. With respect to the latter, two observers
answer my query in the affirmative, but enter into no details. Mr. Taplin, however, appends to my descriptive
remarks the words "this is exact." With respect to negroes, the lady who told me of Fra Angelico's picture,
saw a negro towing a boat on the Nile, and as he encountered an obstruction, she observed his griefmuscles
in strong action, with the middle of the forehead well wrinkled. Mr. Geach watched a Malay man in Malacca,
with the corners of his mouth much depressed, the eyebrows oblique, with deep short grooves on the
forehead. This expression lasted for a very short time; and Mr. Geach remarks it "was a strange one, very
much like a person about to cry at some great loss."
In India Mr. H. Erskine found that the natives were familiar with this expression; and Mr. J. Scott, of the
Botanic Gardens, Calcutta, has obligingly sent me a full description of two cases. He observed during some
time, himself unseen, a very young Dhangar woman from Nagpore, the wife of one of the gardeners, nursing
her baby who was at the point of death; and he distinctly saw the eyebrows raised at the inner corners, the
eyelids drooping, the forehead wrinkled in the middle, the mouth slightly open, with the corners much
depressed. He then came from behind a screen of plants and spoke to the poor woman, who started, burst into
a bitter flood of tears, and besought him to cure her baby. The second case was that of a Hindustani man, who
from illness and poverty was compelled to sell his favourite goat. After receiving the money, he repeatedly
looked at the money in his hand and then at the goat, as if doubting whether he would not return it. He went
to the goat, which was tied up ready to be led away, and the animal reared up and licked his hands. His eyes
then wavered from side to side; his "mouth was partially closed, with the corners very decidedly depressed."
At last the poor man seemed to make up his mind that he must part with his goat, and then, as Mr. Scott saw,
the eyebrows became slightly oblique, with the characteristic puckering or swelling at the inner ends, but the
wrinkles on the forehead were not present. The man stood thus for a minute, then heaving a deep sigh, burst
into tears, raised up his two hands, blessed the goat, turned round, and without looking again, went away.
_On the cause of the obliquity of the eyebrows under suffering_. During several years no expression
seemed to me so utterly perplexing as this which we are here considering. Why should grief or anxiety cause
the central fasciae alone of the frontal muscle together with those round the eyes, to contract? Here we seem
to have a complex movement for the sole purpose of expressing grief; and yet it is a comparatively rare
expression, and often overlooked. I believe the explanation is not so difficult as it at first appears. Dr.
Duchenne gives a photograph of the young man before referred to, who, when looking upwards at a strongly
illuminated surface, involuntarily contracted his griefmuscles in an exaggerated manner. I had entirely
forgotten this photograph, when on a very bright day with the sun behind me, I met, whilst on horseback, a
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 71
Page No 74
girl whose eyebrows, as she looked up at me, became extremely oblique, with the proper furrows on her
forehead. I have observed the same movement under similar circumstances on several subsequent occasions.
On my return home I made three of my children, without giving them any clue to my object, look as long and
as attentively as they could, at the summit of a tall tree standing against an extremely bright sky. With all
three, the orbicular, corrugator, and pyramidal muscles were energetically contracted, through reflex action,
from the excitement of the retina, so that their eyes might be protected from the bright light. But they tried
their utmost to look upwards; and now a curious struggle, with spasmodic twitchings, could be observed
between the whole or only the central portion of the frontal muscle, and the several muscles which serve to
lower the eyebrows and close the eyelids. The involuntary contraction of the pyramidal caused the basal part
of their noses to be transversely and deeply wrinkled. In one of the three children, the whole eyebrows were
momentarily raised and lowered by the alternate contraction of the whole frontal muscle and of the muscles
surrounding the eyes, so that the whole breadth of the forehead was alternately wrinkled and smoothed. In the
other two children the forehead became wrinkled in the middle part alone, rectangular furrows being thus
produced; and the eyebrows were rendered oblique, with their inner extremities puckered and swollen, in
the one child in a slight degree, in the other in a strongly marked manner. This difference in the obliquity of
the eyebrows apparently depended on a difference in their general mobility, and in the strength of the
pyramidal muscles. In both these cases the eyebrows and forehead were acted on under the influence of a
strong light, in precisely the same manner, in every characteristic detail, as under the influence of grief or
anxiety.
Duchenne states that the pyramidal muscle of the nose is less under the control of the will than are the other
muscles round the eyes. He remarks that the young man who could so well act on his griefmuscles, as well
as on most of his other facial muscles, could not contract the pyramidals.[5] This power, however, no doubt
differs in different persons. The pyramidal muscle serves to draw down the skin of the forehead between the
eyebrows, together with their inner extremities. The central fasciae of the frontal are the antagonists of the
pyramidal; and if the action of the latter is to be specially checked, these central fasciae must be contracted.
So that with persons having powerful pyramidal muscles, if there is under the influence of a bright light an
unconscious desire to prevent the lowering of the eyebrows, the central fasciae of the frontal muscle must be
brought into play; and their contraction, if sufficiently strong to overmaster the pyramidals, together with the
contraction of the corrugator and orbicular muscles, will act in the manner just described on the eyebrows and
forehead.
When children scream or cry out, they contract, as we know, the orbicular, corrugator, and pyramidal
muscles, primarily for the sake of compressing their eyes, and thus protecting them from being gorged with
blood, and secondarily through habit. I therefore expected to find with children, that when they endeavoured
either to prevent a cryingfit from coming on, or to stop crying, they would cheek the contraction of the
abovenamed muscles, in the same manner as when looking upwards at a bright light; and consequently that
the central fasciae of the frontal muscle would often be brought into play. Accordingly, I began myself to
observe children at such times, and asked others, including some medical men, to do the same. It is necessary
to observe carefully, as the peculiar opposed action of these muscles is not nearly so plain in children, owing
to their foreheads not easily wrinkling, as in adults. But I soon found that the griefmuscles were very
frequently brought into distinct action on these occasions. It would be superfluous to give all the cases which
have been observed; and I will specify only a few. A little girl, a year and a half old, was teased by some
other children, and before bursting into tears her eyebrows became decidedly oblique. With an older girl the
same obliquity was observed, with the inner ends of the eyebrows plainly puckered; and at the same time the
corners of the mouth were drawn downwards. As soon as she burst into tears, the features all changed and
this peculiar expression vanished. Again, after a little boy had been vaccinated, which made him scream and
cry violently, the surgeon gave him an orange brought for the purpose, and this pleased the child much; as he
stopped crying all the characteristic movements were observed, including the formation of rectangular
wrinkles in the middle of the forehead. Lastly, I met on the road a little girl three or four years old, who had
been frightened by a dog, and when I asked her what was the matter, she stopped whimpering, and her
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 72
Page No 75
eyebrows instantly became oblique to an extraordinary degree.
[5] Mecanisme de la Phys. Humaine, Album, p. 15.
Here then, as I cannot doubt, we have the key to the problem why the central fasciae of the frontal muscle and
the muscles round the eyes contract in opposition to each other under the influence of grief;whether their
contraction be prolonged, as with the melancholic insane, or momentary, from some trifling cause of distress.
We have all of us, as infants, repeatedly contracted our orbicular, corrugator, and pyramidal muscles, in order
to protect our eyes whilst screaming; our progenitors before us have done the same during many generations;
and though with advancing years we easily prevent, when feeling distressed, the utterance of screams, we
cannot from long habit always prevent a slight contraction of the abovenamed muscles; nor indeed do we
observe their contraction in ourselves, or attempt to stop it, if slight. But the pyramidal muscles seem to be
less under the command of the will than the other related muscles; and if they be well developed, their
contraction can be checked only by the antagonistic contraction of the central fasciae of the frontal muscle.
The result which necessarily follows, if these fasciae contract energetically, is the oblique drawing up of the
eyebrows, the puckering of their inner ends, and the formation of rectangular furrows on the middle of the
forehead. As children and women cry much more freely than men, and as grownup persons of both sexes
rarely weep except from mental distress, we can understand why the griefmuscles are more frequently seen
in action, as I believe to be the case, with children and women than with men; and with adults of both sexes
from mental distress alone. In some of the cases before recorded, as in that of the poor Dhangar woman and
of the Hindustani man, the action of the griefmuscles was quickly followed by bitter weeping. In all cases of
distress, whether great or small, our brains tend through long habit to send an order to certain muscles to
contract, as if we were still infants on the point of screaming out; but this order we, by the wondrous power of
the will, and through habit, are able partially to counteract; although this is effected unconsciously, as far as
the means of counteraction are concerned.
_On the depression of the corners of the mouth_.This action is effected by the _depressores anguili oris_
(see letter K in figs. 1 and 2). The fibres of this muscle diverge downwards, with the upper convergent ends
attached round the angles of the mouth, and to the lower lip a little way within the angles.[6] Some of the
fibres appear to be antagonistic to the great zygomatic muscle, and others to the several muscles running to
the outer part of the upper lip. The contraction of this muscle draws downwards and outwards the corners of
the mouth, including the outer part of the upper lip, and even in a slight degree the wings of the nostrils.
When the mouth is closed and this muscle acts, the commissure or line of junction of the two lips forms a
curved line with the concavity downwards,[7] and the lips themselves are generally somewhat protruded,
especially the lower one. The mouth in this state is well represented in the two photographs (Plate II., figs. 6
and 7) by Mr. Rejlander. The upper boy (fig. 6) had just stopped crying, after receiving a slap on the face
from another boy; and the right moment was seized for photographing him.
[6] Henle, Handbuch der Anat. des Menschen, 1858, B. i. s. 148, figs. 68 and 69.
[7] See the account of the action of this muscle by Dr. Duchenne, `Mecanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,
Album (1862), viii. p. 34.
The expression of low spirits, grief or dejection, due to the contraction of this muscle has been noticed by
every one who has written on the subject. To say that a person "is down in the mouth," is synonymous with
saying that he is out of spirits. The depression of the corners may often be seen, as already stated on the
authority of Dr. Crichton Browne and Mr. Nicol, with the melancholic insane, and was well exhibited in
some photographs sent to me by the former gentleman, of patients with a strong tendency to suicide. It has
been observed with men belonging to various races, namely with Hindoos, the dark hilltribes of India,
Malays, and, as the Rev. Mr. Hagenauer informs me, with the aborigines of Australia.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 73
Page No 76
When infants scream they firmly contract the muscles round their eyes, and this draws up the upper lip; and
as they have to keep their mouths widely open, the depressor muscles running to the corners are likewise
brought into strong action. This generally, but not invariably, causes a slight angular bend in the lower lip on
both sides, near the corners of the mouth. The result of the upper and lower lip being thus acted on is that the
mouth assumes a squarish outline. The contraction of the depressor muscle is best seen in infants when not
screaming violently, and especially just before they begin, or when they cease to scream. Their little faces
then acquire an extremely piteous expression, as I continually observed with my own infants between the
ages of about six weeks and two or three months. Sometimes, when they are struggling against a cryingfit,
the outline of the mouth is curved in so exaggerated a manner as to be like a horseshoe; and the expression of
misery then becomes a ludicrous caricature.
The explanation of the contraction of this muscle, under the influence of low spirits or dejection, apparently
follows from the same general principles as in the case of the obliquity of the eyebrows. Dr. Duchenne
informs me that he concludes from his observations, now prolonged during many years, that this is one of the
facial muscles which is least under the control of the will. This fact may indeed be inferred from what has just
been stated with respect to infants when doubtfully beginning to cry, or endeavouring to stop crying; for they
then generally command all the other facial muscles more effectually than they do the depressors of the
corners of the mouth. Two excellent observers who had no theory on the subject, one of them a surgeon,
carefully watched for me some older children and women as with some opposed struggling they very
gradually approached the point of bursting out into tears; and both observers felt sure that the depressors
began to act before any of the other muscles. Now as the depressors have been repeatedly brought into strong
action during infancy in many generations, nerveforce will tend to flow, on the principle of long associated
habit, to these muscles as well as to various other facial muscles, whenever in after life even a slight feeling
of distress is experienced. But as the depressors are somewhat less under the control of the will than most of
the other muscles, we might expect that they would often slightly contract, whilst the others remained
passive. It is remarkable how small a depression of the corners of the mouth gives to the countenance an
expression of low spirits or dejection, so that an extremely slight contraction of these muscles would be
sufficient to betray this state of mind.
I may here mention a trifling observation, as it will serve to sum up our present subject. An old lady with a
comfortable but absorbed expression sat nearly opposite to me in a railway carriage. Whilst I was looking at
her, I saw that her _depressores anguli oris_ became very slightly, yet decidedly, contracted; but as her
countenance remained as placid as ever, I reflected how meaningless was this contraction, and how easily one
might be deceived. The thought had hardly occurred to me when I saw that her eyes suddenly became
suffused with tears almost to overflowing, and her whole countenance fell. There could now be no doubt that
some painful recollection, perhaps that of a longlost child, was passing through her mind. As soon as her
sensorium was thus affected, certain nervecells from long habit instantly transmitted an order to all the
respiratory muscles, and to those round the mouth, to prepare for a fit of crying. But the order was
countermanded by the will, or rather by a later acquired habit, and all the muscles were obedient, excepting in
a slight degree the _depressores anguli oris_. The mouth was not even opened; the respiration was not
hurried; and no muscle was affected except those which draw down the corners of the mouth.
As soon as the mouth of this lady began, involuntarily and unconsciously on her part, to assume the proper
form for a cryingfit, we may feel almost sure that some nerveinfluence would have been transmitted
through the long accustomed channels to the various respiratory muscles, as well as to those round the eyes,
and to the vasomotor centre which governs the supply of blood sent to the lacrymal glands. Of this latter
fact we have indeed clear evidence in her eyes becoming slightly suffused with tears; and we can understand
this, as the lacrymal glands are less under the control of the will than the facial muscles. No doubt there
existed at the same time some tendency in the muscles round the eyes at contract, as if for the sake of
protecting them from being gorged with blood, but this contraction was completely overmastered, and her
brow remained unruffled. Had the pyramidal, corrugator, and orbicular muscles been as little obedient to the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR. 74
Page No 77
will, as they are in many persons, they would have been slightly acted on; and then the central fasciae of the
frontal muscle would have contracted in antagonism, and her eyebrows would have become oblique, with
rectangular furrows on her forehead. Her countenance would then have expressed still more plainly than it
did a state of dejection, or rather one of grief.
Through steps such as these we can understand how it is, that as soon as some melancholy thought passes
through the brain, there occurs a just perceptible drawing down of the corners of the mouth, or a slight raising
up of the inner ends of the eyebrows, or both movements combined, and immediately afterwards a slight
suffusion of tears. A thrill of nerveforce is transmitted along several habitual channels, and produces an
effect on any point where the will has not acquired through long habit much power of interference. The above
actions may be considered as rudimental vestiges of the screamingfits, which are so frequent and prolonged
during infancy. In this case, as well as in many others, the links are indeed wonderful which connect cause
and effect in giving rise to various expressions on the human countenance; and they explain to us the
meaning of certain movements, which we involuntarily and unconsciously perform, whenever certain
transitory emotions pass through our minds.
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS,
DEVOTION.
Laughter primarily the expression of joyLudicrous ideas Movements of the features during
laughterNature of the sound producedThe secretion of tears during loud laughter Gradation from
loud laughter to gentle smilingHigh spirits The expression of loveTender feelingsDevotion.
JOY, when intense, leads to various purposeless movements to dancing about, clapping the hands,
stamping, and to loud laughter. Laughter seems primarily to be the expression of mere joy or happiness. We
clearly see this in children at play, who are almost incessantly laughing. With young persons past childhood,
when they are in high spirits, there is always much meaningless laughter. The laughter of the gods is
described by Homer as "the exuberance of their celestial joy after their daily banquet." A man smilesand
smiling, as we shall see, graduates into laughter at meeting an old friend in the street, as he does at any
trifling pleasure, such as smelling a sweet perfume.[1] Laura Bridgman, from her blindness and deafness,
could not have acquired any expression through imitation, yet when a letter from a beloved friend was
communicated to her by gesturelanguage, she "laughed and clapped her hands, and the colour mounted to
her cheeks." On other occasions she has been seen to stamp for joy.[2]
[1] Herbert Spencer, `Essays Scientific,' 1858, p. 360.
Idiots and imbecile persons likewise afford good evidence that laughter or smiling primarily expresses mere
happiness or joy. Dr. Crichton Browne, to whom, as on so many other occasions, I am indebted for the results
of his wide experience, informs me that with idiots laughter is the most prevalent and frequent of all the
emotional expressions. Many idiots are morose, passionate, restless, in a painful state of mind, or utterly
stolid, and these never laugh. Others frequently laugh in a quite senseless manner. Thus an idiot boy,
incapable of speech, complained to Dr. Browne, by the aid of signs, that another boy in the asylum had given
him a black eye; and this was accompanied by "explosions of laughter and with his face covered with the
broadest smiles." There is another large class of idiots who are persistently joyous and benign, and who are
constantly laughing or smiling.[3] Their countenances often exhibit a stereotyped smile; their joyousness is
increased, and they grin, chuckle, or giggle, whenever food is placed before them, or when they are caressed,
are shown bright colours, or hear music. Some of them laugh more than usual when they walk about, or
attempt any muscular exertion. The joyousness of most of these idiots cannot possibly be associated, as Dr.
Browne remarks, with any distinct ideas: they simply feel pleasure, and express it by laughter or smiles. With
imbeciles rather higher in the scale, personal vanity seems to be the commonest cause of laughter, and next to
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 75
Page No 78
this, pleasure arising from the approbation of their conduct.
[2] F. Lieber on the vocal sounds of L. Bridgman, `Smithsonian Contributions,' 1851, vol. ii. p. 6.
[3] See, also, Mr. Marshall, in Phil. Transact. 1864, p. 526.
With grownup persons laughter is excited by causes considerably different from those which suffice during
childhood; but this remark hardly applies to smiling. Laughter in this respect is analogous with weeping,
which with adults is almost confined to mental distress, whilst with children it is excited by bodily pain or
any suffering, as well as by fear or rage. Many curious discussions have been written on the causes of
laughter with grownup persons. The subject is extremely complex. Something incongruous or
unaccountable, exciting surprise and some sense of superiority in the laugher, who must be in a happy frame
of mind, seems to be the commonest cause.[4] The circumstances must not be of a momentous nature: no
poor man would laugh or smile on suddenly hearing that a large fortune had been bequeathed to him. If the
mind is strongly excited by pleasurable feelings, and any little unexpected event or thought occurs, then, as
Mr. Herbert Spencer remarks,[5] "a large amount of nervous energy, instead of being allowed to expend itself
in producing an equivalent amount of the new thoughts and emotion which were nascent, is suddenly checked
in its flow." . . . "The excess must discharge itself in some other direction, and there results an efflux through
the motor nerves to various classes of the muscles, producing the halfconvulsive actions we term laughter."
An observation, bearing on this point, was made by a correspondent during the recent siege of Paris, namely,
that the German soldiers. after strong excitement from exposure to extreme danger, were particularly apt to
burst out into loud laughter at the smallest joke. So again when young children are just beginning to cry, an
unexpected event will sometimes suddenly turn their crying into laughter, which apparently serves equally
well to expend their superfluous nervous energy.
[4] Mr. Bain (`The Emotions and the Will,' 1865, p. 247) has a long and interesting discussion on the
Ludicrous. The quotation above given about the laughter of the gods is taken from this work. See, also,
Mandeville, `The Fable of the Bees,' vol. ii. p. 168.
[5] `The Physiology of Laughter,' Essays, Second Series, 1863, p. 114.
The imagination is sometimes said to be tickled by a ludicrous idea; and this socalled tickling of the mind is
curiously analogous with that of the body. Every one knows how immoderately children laugh, and how their
whole bodies are convulsed when they are tickled. The anthropoid apes, as we have seen, likewise utter a
reiterated sound, corresponding with our laughter, when they are tickled, especially under the armpits. I
touched with a bit of paper the sole of the foot of one of my infants, when only seven days old, and it was
suddenly jerked away and the toes curled about, as in an older child. Such movements, as well as laughter
from being tickled, are manifestly reflex actions; and this is likewise shown by the minute unstriped muscles,
which serve to erect the separate hairs on the body, contracting near a tickled surface.[6] Yet laughter from a
ludicrous idea, though involuntary, cannot be called a strictly reflex action. In this case, and in that of
laughter from being tickled, the mind must be in a pleasurable condition; a young child, if tickled by a
strange man, would scream from fear. The touch must be light, and an idea or event, to be ludicrous, must not
be of grave import. The parts of the body which are most easily tickled are those which are not commonly
touched, such as the armpits or between the toes, or parts such as the soles of the feet, which are habitually
touched by a broad surface; but the surface on which we sit offers a marked exception to this rule. According
to Gratiolet,[7] certain nerves are much more sensitive to tickling than others. From the fact that a child can
hardly tickle itself, or in a much less degree than when tickled by another person, it seems that the precise
point to be touched must not be known; so with the mind, something unexpected a novel or incongruous
idea which breaks through an habitual train of thought appears to he a strong element in the ludicrous.
[6] J. Lister in `Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science,' 1853, vol. 1. p. 266.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 76
Page No 79
The sound of laughter is produced by a deep inspiration followed by short, interrupted, spasmodic
contractions of the chest, and especially of the diaphragm.[8] Hence we hear of "laughter holding both his
sides." From the shaking of the body, the head nods to and fro. The lower jaw often quivers up and down, as
is likewise the case with some species of baboons, when they are much pleased.
During laughter the mouth is opened more or less widely, with the corners drawn much backwards, as well as
a little upwards; and the upper lip is somewhat raised. The drawing back of the corners is best seen in
moderate laughter, and especially in a broad smile the latter epithet showing how the mouth is widened. In
the accompanying figs. 13, Plate III., different degrees of moderate laughter and smiling have been
photographed. The figure of the little girl, with the hat is by Dr. Wallich, and the expression was a genuine
one; the other two are by Mr. Rejlander. Dr. Duchenne repeatedly insists[9] that, under the emotion of joy,
the mouth is acted on exclusively by the great zygomatic muscles, which serve to draw the corners backwards
and upwards; but judging from the manner in which the upper teeth are always exposed during laughter and
broad smiling, as well as from my own sensations, I cannot doubt that some of the muscles running to the
upper lip are likewise brought into moderate action. The upper and lower orbicular muscles of the eyes are at
the same time more or less contracted; and there is an intimate connection, as explained in the chapter on
weeping, between the orbiculars, especially the lower ones and some of the muscles running to the upper lip.
Henle remarks[10] on this head, that when a man closely shuts one eye he cannot avoid retracting the upper
lip on the same side; conversely, if any one will place his finger on his lower eyelid, and then uncover his
upper incisors as much as possible, he will feel, as his upper lip is drawn strongly upwards, that the muscles
of the lower eyelid contract. In Henle's drawing, given in woodcut, fig. 2, the _musculus malaris_ (H) which
runs to the upper lip may be seen to form an almost integral part of the lower orbicular muscle.
[7] `De la Physionomie,' p. 186.
[8] Sir C. Bell (Anat. of Expression, p. 147) makes some remarks on the movement of the diaphragm during
laughter.
[9] `Mecanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,' Album, Legende vi.
Dr. Duchenne has given a large photograph of an old man (reduced on Plate III. fig 4), in his usual passive
condition, and another of the same man (fig. 5), naturally smiling. The latter was instantly recognized by
every one to whom it was shown as true to nature. He has also given, as an example of an unnatural or false
smile, another photograph (fig. 6) of the same old man, with the corners of his mouth strongly retracted by
the galvanization of the great zygomatic muscles. That the expression is not natural is clear, for I showed this
photograph to twentyfour persons, of whom three could not in the least tell what was meant, whilst the
others, though they perceived that the expression was of the nature of a smile, answered in such words as "a
wicked joke," "trying to laugh," "grinning laughter ... .. halfamazed laughter," Dr. Duchenne attributes the
falseness of the expression altogether to the orbicular muscles of the lower eyelids not being sufficiently
contracted; for he justly lays great stress on their contraction in the expression of joy. No doubt there is much
truth in this view, but not, as it appears to me, the whole truth. The contraction of the lower orbiculars is
always accompanied, as we have seen, by the drawing up of the upper lip. Had the upper lip, in fig. 6, been
thus acted on to a slight extent, its curvature would have been less rigid, the nasolabial farrow would have
been slightly different, and the whole expression would, as I believe, have been more natural, independently
of the more conspicuous effect from the stronger contraction of the lower eyelids. The corruptor muscle,
moreover, in fig. 6, is too much contracted, causing a frown; and this muscle never acts under the influence of
joy except during strongly pronounced or violent laughter.
[10] Handbuch der System. Anat. des Menschen, 1858, B. i. s. 144. See my woodcut (H. fig. 2).
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 77
Page No 80
By the drawing backwards and upwards of the corners of the mouth, through the contraction of the great
zygomatic muscles, and by the raising of the upper lip, the cheeks are drawn upwards. Wrinkles are thus
formed under the eyes, and, with old people, at their outer ends; and these are highly characteristic of laughter
or smiling. As a gentle smile increases into a strong one, or into a laugh, every one may feel and see, if he
will attend to his own sensations and look at himself in a mirror, that as the upper lip is drawn up and the
lower orbiculars contract, the wrinkles in the lower eyelids and those beneath the eyes are much strengthened
or increased. At the same time, as I have repeatedly observed, the eyebrows are slightly lowered, which
shows that the upper as well as the lower orbiculars contract at least to some degree, though this passes
unperecived, as far as our sensations are concerned. If the original photograph of the old man, with his
countenance in its usual placid state (fig. 4), be compared with that (fig. 5) in which he is naturally smiling, it
may be seen that the eyebrows in the latter are a little lowered. I presume that this is owing to the upper
orbiculars being impelled, through the force of longassociated habit, to act to a certain extent in concert with
the lower orbiculars, which themselves contract in connection with the drawing up of the upper lip.
The tendency in the zygomatic muscles to contract under pleasurable emotions is shown by a curious fact,
communicated to me by Dr. Browne, with respect to patients suffering from GENERAL PARALYSIS OF
THE INSANE.[11] "In this malady there is almost invariably optimismdelusions as to wealth, rank,
grandeurinsane joyousness, benevolence, and profusion, while its very earliest physical symptom is
trembling at the corners of the mouth and at the outer corners of the eyes. This is a wellrecognized fact.
Constant tremulous agitation of the inferior palpebral and great zygomatic muscles is pathognomic of the
earlier stages of general paralysis. The countenance has a pleased and benevolent expression. As the disease
advances other muscles become involved, but until complete fatuity is reached, the prevailing expression is
that of feeble benevolence."
As in laughing and broadly smiling the cheeks and upper lip are much raised, the nose appears to be
shortened, and the skin on the bridge becomes finely wrinkled in transverse lines, with other oblique
longitudinal lines on the sides. The upper front teeth are commonly exposed. A wellmarked nasolabial fold
is formed, which runs from the wing of each nostril to the corner of the mouth; and this fold is often double
in old persons.
[11] See, also, remarks to the same effect by Dr. J. Crichton Browne in `Journal of Mental Science,' April,
1871, p. 149.
A bright and sparkling eye is as characteristic of a pleased or amused state of mind, as is the retraction of the
corners of the mouth and upper lip with the wrinkles thus produced. Even the eyes of microcephalous idiots,
who are so degraded that they never learn to speak, brighten slightly when they are pleased.[12] Under
extreme laughter the eyes are too much suffused with tears to sparkle; but the moisture squeezed out of the
glands during moderate laughter or smiling may aid in giving them lustre; though this must be of altogether
subordinate importance, as they become dull from grief, though they are then often moist. Their brightness
seems to be chiefly due to their tenseness,[13] owing to the contraction of the orbicular muscles and to the
pressure of the raised cheeks. But, according to Dr. Piderit, who has discussed this point more fully than any
other writer,[14] the tenseness may be largely attributed to the eyeballs becoming filled with blood and other
fluids, from the acceleration of the circulation, consequent on the excitement of pleasure. He remarks on the
contrast in the appearance of the eyes of a hectic patient with a rapid circulation, and of a man suffering from
cholera with almost all the fluids of his body drained from him. Any cause which lowers the circulation
deadens the eye. I remember seeing a man utterly prostrated by prolonged and severe exertion during a very
hot day, and a bystander compared his eyes to those of a boiled codfish.
[12] C. Vogt, `Memoire sur les Microcephales,' 1867, p. 21.
[13] Sir C. Bell, `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 133.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 78
Page No 81
[14] `Mimik und Physiognomik,' 1867, s. 6367.
To return to the sounds produced during laughter. We can see in a vague manner how the utterance of sounds
of some kind would naturally become associated with a pleasurable state of mind; for throughout a large part
of the animal kingdom vocal or instrumental sounds are employed either as a call or as a charm by one sex
for the other. They are also employed as the means for a joyful meeting between the parents and their
offspring, and between the attached members of the same social community. But why the sounds which man
utters when he is pleased have the peculiar reiterated character of laughter we do not know. Nevertheless we
can see that they would naturally be as different as possible from the screams or cries of distress; and as in the
production of the latter, the expirations are prolonged and continuous, with the inspirations short and
interrupted, so it might perhaps have been expected with the sounds uttered from joy, that the expirations
would have been short and broken with the inspirations prolonged; and this is the case.
It is an equally obscure point why the corners of the mouth are retracted and the upper lip raised during
ordinary laughter. The mouth must not be opened to its utmost extent, for when this occurs during a
paroxysm of excessive laughter hardly any sound is emitted; or it changes its tone and seems to come from
deep down in the throat. The respiratory muscles, and even those of the limbs, are at the same time thrown
into rapid vibratory movements. The lower jaw often partakes of this movement, and this would tend to
prevent the mouth from being widely opened. But as a full volume of sound has to be poured forth, the orifice
of the mouth must be large; and it is perhaps to gain this end that the corners are retracted and the upper lip
raised. Although we can hardly account for the shape of the mouth during laughter, which leads to wrinkles
being formed beneath the eyes, nor for the peculiar reiterated sound of laughter, nor for the quivering of the
jaws, nevertheless we may infer that all these effects are due to some common cause. For they are all
characteristic and expressive of a pleased state of mind in various kinds of monkeys.
A graduated series can be followed from violent to moderate laughter, to a broad smile, to a gentle smile, and
to the expression of mere cheerfulness. During excessive laughter the whole body is often thrown backward
and shakes, or is almost convulsed; the respiration is much disturbed; the head and face become gorged with
blood, with the veins distended; and the orbicular muscles are spasmodically contracted in order to protect the
eyes. Tears are freely shed. Hence, as formerly remarked, it is scarcely possible to point out any difference
between the tearstained face of a person after a paroxysm of excessive laughter and after a bitter
cryingfit.[15] It is probably due to the close similarity of the spasmodic movements caused by these widely
different emotions that hysteric patients alternately cry and laugh with violence, and that young children
sometimes pass suddenly from the one to the other state. Mr. Swinhoe informs me that he has often seen the
Chinese, when suffering from deep grief, burst out into hysterical fits of laughter.
[15] Sir T. Reynolds remarks (`Discourses,' xii. p. 100), it is curious to observe, and it is certainly true, that
the extremes of contrary passions are, with very little variation, expressed by the same action." He gives as an
instance the frantic joy of a Bacchante and the grief of a Mary Magdalen.
I was anxious to know whether tears are freely shed during excessive laughter by most of the races of men,
and I hear from my correspondents that this is the case. One instance was observed with the Hindoos, and
they themselves said that it often occurred. So it is with the Chinese. The women of a wild tribe of Malays in
the Malacca peninsula, sometimes shed tears when they laugh heartily, though this seldom occurs. With the
Dyaks of Borneo it must frequently be the case, at least with the women, for I hear from the Rajah C. Brooke
that it is a common expression with them to say "we nearly made tears from laughter." The aborigines of
Australia express their emotions freely, and they are described by my correspondents as jumping about and
clapping their hands for joy, and as often roaring with laughter. No less than four observers have seen their
eyes freely watering on such occasions; and in one instance the tears rolled down their cheeks. Mr. Bulmer, a
missionary in a remote part of Victoria, remarks, "that they have a keen sense of the ridiculous; they are
excellent mimics, and when one of them is able to imitate the peculiarities of some absent member of the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 79
Page No 82
tribe, it is very common to hear all in the camp convulsed with laughter." With Europeans hardly anything
excites laughter so easily as mimicry; and it is rather curious to find the same fact with the savages of
Australia, who constitute one of the most distinct races in the world.
In Southern Africa with two tribes of Kafirs, especially with the women, their eyes often fill with tears during
laughter. Gaika, the brother of the chief Sandilli, answers my query on this bead, with the words, "Yes, that is
their common practice." Sir Andrew Smith has seen the painted face of a Hottentot woman all furrowed with
tears after a fit of laughter. In Northern Africa, with the Abyssinians, tears are secreted under the same
circumstances. Lastly, in North America, the same fact has been observed in a remarkably savage and
isolated tribe, but chiefly with the women; in another tribe it was observed only on a single occasion.
Excessive laughter, as before remarked, graduates into moderate laughter. In this latter case the muscles
round the eyes are much less contracted, and there is little or no frowning. Between a gentle laugh and a
broad smile there is hardly any difference, excepting that in smiling no reiterated sound is uttered, though a
single rather strong expiration, or slight noise a rudiment of a laughmay often be heard at the
commencement of a smile. On a moderately smiling countenance the contraction of the upper orbicular
muscles can still just be traced by a slight lowering of the eyebrows. The contraction of the lower orbicular
and palpebral muscles is much plainer, and is shown by the wrinkling of the lower eyelids and of the skin
beneath them, together with a slight drawing up of the upper lip. From the broadest smile we pass by the
finest steps into the gentlest one. In this latter case the features are moved in a much less degree, and much
more slowly, and the mouth is kept closed. The curvature of the nasolabial furrow is also slightly different
in the two cases. We thus see that no abrupt line of demarcation can be drawn between the movement of the
features during the most violent laughter and a very faint smile.[16]
A smile, therefore, may be said to be the first stage in the development of a laugh. But a different and more
probable view may be suggested; namely, that the habit of uttering load reiterated sounds from a sense of
pleasure, first led to the retraction of the corners of the mouth and of the upper lip, and to the contraction of
the orbicular muscles; and that now, through association and longcontinued habit, the same muscles are
brought into slight play whenever any cause excites in us a feeling which, if stronger, would have led to
laughter; and the result is a smile.
[16] Dr. Piderit has come to the same conclusion, ibid. s. 99.
Whether we look at laughter as the full development of a smile, or, as is more probable, at a gentle smile as
the last trace of a habit, firmly fixed during many generations, of laughing whenever we are joyful, we can
follow in our infants the gradual passage of the one into the other. It is well known to those who have the
charge of young infants, that it is difficult to feel sure when certain movements about their mouths are really
expressive; that is, when they really smile. Hence I carefully watched my own infants. One of them at the age
of fortyfive days, and being at the time in a happy frame of mind, smiled; that is, the corners of the mouth
were retracted, and simultaneously the eyes became decidedly bright. I observed the same thing on the
following day; but on the third day the child was not quite well and there was no trace of a smile, and this
renders it probable that the previous smiles were real. Eight days subsequently and during the next
succeeding week, it was remarkable how his eyes brightened whenever he smiled, and his nose became at the
same time transversely wrinkled. This was now accompanied by a little bleating noise, which perhaps
represented a laugh. At the age of 113 days these little noises, which were always made during expiration,
assumed a slightly different character, and were more broken or interrupted, as in sobbing; and this was
certainly incipient laughter. The change in tone seemed to me at the time to be connected with the greater
lateral extension of the mouth as the smiles became broader.
In a second infant the first real smile was observed at about the same age, viz. fortyfive days; and in a third,
at a somewhat earlier age. The second infant, when sixtyfive days old, smiled much more broadly and
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 80
Page No 83
plainly than did the one first mentioned at the same age; and even at this early age uttered noises very like
laughter. In this gradual acquirement, by infants, of the habit of laughing, we have a case in some degree
analogous to that of weeping. As practice is requisite with the ordinary movements of the body, such as
walking, so it seems to be with laughing and weeping. The art of screaming, on the other hand, from being of
service to infants, has become finely developed from the earliest days.
_High spirits, cheerfulness_.A man in high spirits, though he may not actually smile, commonly exhibits
some tendency to the retraction of the corners of his mouth. From the excitement of pleasure, the circulation
becomes more rapid; the eyes are bright, and the colour of the face rises. The brain, being stimulated by the
increased flow of blood, reacts on the mental powers; lively ideas pass still more rapidly through the mind,
and the affections are warmed. I heard a child, a little under four years old, when asked what was meant by
being in good spirits, answer, "It is laughing, talking, and kissing." It would be difficult to give a truer and
more practical definition. A man in this state holds his body erect, his head upright, and his eyes open. There
is no drooping of the features, and no contraction of the eyebrows. On the contrary, the frontal muscle, as
Moreau observes,[17] tends to contract slightly; and this smooths the brow, removes every trace of a frown,
arches the eyebrows a little, and raises the eyelids. Hence the Latin phrase, _exporrigere frontem_ to
unwrinkle the browmeans, to be cheerful or merry. The whole expression of a man in good spirits is
exactly the opposite of that of one suffering from sorrow. According to Sir C. Bell, "In all the exhilarating
emotions the eyebrows, eyelids, the nostrils, and the angles of the mouth are raised. In the depressing
passions it is the reverse." Under the influence of the latter the brow is heavy, the eyelids, cheeks, mouth, and
whole head droop; the eyes are dull; the countenance pallid, and the respiration slow. In joy the face expands,
in grief it lengthens. Whether the principle of antithesis has here come into play in producing these opposite
expressions, in aid of the direct causes which have been specified and which are sufficiently plain, I will not
pretend to say.
[17] `La Physionomie,' par G. Lavater, edit. of 1820, vol. iv. p. 224. See, also, Sir C. Bell, `Anatomy of
Expression,' p. 172, for the quotation given below.
With all the races of man the expression of good spirit appears to be the same, and is easily recognized. My
informants, from various parts of the Old and New Worlds, answer in the affirmative to my queries on this
head, and they give some particulars with respect to Hindoos, Malays, and New Zealanders. The brightness
of the eyes of the Australians has struck four observers, and the same fact has been noticed with Hindoos,
New Zealanders, and the Dyaks of Borneo.
Savages sometimes express their satisfaction not only by smiling, but by gestures derived from the pleasure
of eating. Thus Mr. Wedgwood[18] quotes Petherick that the negroes on the Upper Nile began a general
rubbing of their bellies when he displayed his beads; and Leichhardt says that the Australians smacked and
clacked their mouths at the sight of his horses and bullocks, and more especially of his kangaroo dogs. The
Greenlanders, "when they affirm anything with pleasure, suck down air with a certain sound;"[19] and this
may be an imitation of the act of swallowing savoury food.
[18] A `Dictionary of English Etymology,' 2nd edit. 1872, Introduction, p. xliv.
Laughter is suppressed by the firm contraction of the orbicular muscles of the mouth, which prevents the
great zygomatic and other muscles from drawing the lips backwards and upwards. The lower lip is also
sometimes held by the teeth, and this gives a roguish expression to the face, as was observed with the blind
and deaf Laura Bridgman.[20] The great zygomatic muscle is sometimes variable in its course, and I have
seen a young woman in whom the _depressores anguli oris_ were brought into strong action in suppressing a
smile; but this by no means gave to her countenance a melancholy expression, owing to the brightness of her
eyes.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 81
Page No 84
Laughter is frequently employed in a forced manner to conceal or mask some other state of mind, even anger.
We often see persons laughing in order to conceal their shame or shyness. When a person purses up his
mouth, as if to prevent the possibility of a smile, though there is nothing to excite one, or nothing to prevent
its free indulgence, an affected, solemn, or pedantic expression is given; but of such hybrid expressions
nothing more need here be said. In the case of derision, a real or pretended smile or laugh is often blended
with the expression proper to contempt, and this may pass into angry contempt or scorn. In such cases the
meaning of the laugh or smile is to show the offending person that he excites only amusement.
_Love, tender feelings, the emotion of love, for instance that of a mother for her infant, is one of the strongest
of which the mind is capable, it can hardly be said to have any proper or peculiar means of expression; and
this is intelligible, as it has not habitually led to any special line of action. No doubt, as affection is a
pleasurable sensation, it generally causes a gentle smile and some brightening of the eyes. A strong desire to
touch the beloved person is commonly felt; and love is expressed by this means more plainly than by any
other.[21] Hence we long to clasp in our arms those whom we tenderly love. We probably owe this desire to
inherited habit, in association with the nursing and tending of our children, and with the mutual caresses of
lovers.
[19] Crantz, quoted by Tylor, `Primitive Culture,' 1871, Vol. i. P. 169.
[20] F. Lieber, `Smithsonian Contributions,' 1851, vol. ii. p. 7.
With the lower animals we see the same principle of pleasure derived from contact in association with love.
Dogs and cats manifestly take pleasure in rubbing against their masters and mistresses, and in being rubbed
or patted by them. Many kinds of monkeys, as I am assured by the keepers in the Zoological Gardens, delight
in fondling and being fondled by each other, and by persons to whom they are attached. Mr. Bartlett has
described to me the behaviour of two chimpanzees, rather older animals than those generally imported into
this country, when they were first brought together. They sat opposite, touching each other with their much
protruded lips; and the one put his hand on the shoulder of the other. They then mutually folded each other in
their arms. Afterwards they stood up, each with one arm on the shoulder of the other, lifted up their heads,
opened their mouths, and yelled with delight.
[21] Mr. Bain remarks (`Mental and Moral Science,' 1868, p. 239), "Tenderness is a pleasurable emotion,
variously stimulated, whose effort is to draw human beings into mutual embrace."
We Europeans are so accustomed to kissing as a mark of affection, that it might be thought to be innate in
mankind; but this is not the case. Steele was mistaken when he said "Nature was its author, and it began with
the first courtship." Jemmy Button, the Fuegian, told me that this practice was unknown in his land. It is
equally unknown with the New Zealanders, Tahitians, Papuans, Australians, Somals of Africa, and the
Esquimaux." But it is so far innate or natural that it apparently depends on pleasure from close contact with a
beloved person; and it is replaced in various parts of the world, by the rubbing of noses, as with the New
Zealanders and Laplanders, by the rubbing or patting of the arms, breasts, or stomachs, or by one man
striking his own face with the hands or feet of another. Perhaps the practice of blowing, as a mark of
affection, on various parts of the body may depend on the same principle.[23]
The feelings which are called tender are difficult to analyse; they seem to be compounded of affection, joy,
and especially of sympathy. These feelings are in themselves of a pleasurable nature, excepting when pity is
too deep, or horror is aroused, as in hearing of a tortured man or animal. They are remarkable under our
present point of view from so readily exciting the secretion of tears. Many a father and son have wept on
meeting after a long separation, especially if the meeting has been unexpected. No doubt extreme joy by itself
tends to act on the lacrymal glands; but on such occasions as the foregoing vague thoughts of the grief which
would have been felt had the father and son never met, will probably have passed through their minds; and
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 82
Page No 85
grief naturally leads to the secretion of tears. Thus on the return of Ulysses:
"Telemachus
Rose, and clung weeping round his father's breast.
There the pent grief rained o'er them, yearning thus.
* * * * * *
Thus piteously they wailed in sore unrest,
And on their weepings had gone down the day,
But that at last Telemachus found words to say."
_Worsley's Translation of the Odyssey_, Book xvi. st. 27.
So again when Penelope at last recognized her husband:
"Then from her eyelids the quick tears did start
And she ran to him from her place, and threw
Her arms about his neck, and a warm dew
Of kisses poured upon him, and thus spake:"
Book xxiii. st. 27.
[22] Sir J. Lubbock, `Prehistoric Times,' 2nd edit. 1869, p. 552, gives full authorities for these statements.
The quotation from Steele is taken from this work.
[23] See a full acount,{sic} with references, by E. B. Tylor, `Researches into the Early History of Mankind,'
2nd edit. 1870, p. 51.
The vivid recollection of our former home, or of longpast happy days, readily causes the eyes to be suffused
with tears; but here, again, the thought naturally occurs that these days will never return. In such cases we
may be said to sympathize with ourselves in our present, in comparison with our former, state. Sympathy
with the distresses of others, even with the imaginary distresses of a heroine in a pathetic story, for whom we
feel no affection, readily excites tears. So does sympathy with the happiness of others, as with that of a lover,
at last successful after many hard trials in a welltold tale.
Sympathy appears to constitute a separate or distinct emotion; and it is especially apt to excite the lacrymal
glands. This holds good whether we give or receive sympathy. Every one must have noticed how readily
children burst out crying if we pity them for some small hurt. With the melancholic insane, as Dr. Crichton
Browne informs me, a kind word will often plunge them into unrestrained weeping. As soon as we express
our pity for the grief of a friend, tears often come into our own eyes. The feeling of sympathy is commonly
explained by assuming that, when we see or hear of suffering in another, the idea of suffering is called up so
vividly in our own minds that we ourselves suffer. But this explanation is hardly sufficient, for it does not
account for the intimate alliance between sympathy and affection. We undoubtedly sympathize far more
deeply with a beloved than with an indifferent person; and the sympathy of the one gives us far more relief
than that of the other. Yet assuredly we can sympathize with those for whom we feel no affection.
Why suffering, when actually experienced by ourselves, excites weeping, has been discussed in a former
chapter. With respect to joy, its natural and universal expression is laughter; and with all the races of man
loud laughter leads to the secretion of tears more freely than does any other cause excepting distress. The
suffusion of the eyes with tears, which undoubtedly occurs under great joy, though there is no laughter, can,
as it seems to me, be explained through habit and association on the same principles as the effusion of tears
from grief, although there is no screaming. Nevertheless it is not a little remarkable that sympathy with the
distresses of others should excite tears more freely than our own distress; and this certainly is the case. Many
a man, from whose eyes no suffering of his own could wring a tear, has shed tears at the sufferings of a
beloved friend. It is still more remarkable that sympathy with the happiness or good fortune of those whom
we tenderly love should lead to the same result, whilst a similar happiness felt by ourselves would leave our
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 83
Page No 86
eyes dry. We should, however, bear in mind that the longcontinued habit of restraint which is so powerful in
checking the free flow of tears from bodily pain, has not been brought into play in preventing a moderate
effusion of tears in sympathy with the sufferings or happiness of others.
Music has a wonderful power, as I have elsewhere attempted to show,[24] of recalling in a vague and
indefinite manner, those strong emotions which were felt during longpast ages, when, as is probable, our
early progenitors courted each other by the aid of vocal tones. And as several of our strongest
emotionsgrief, great joy, love, and sympathylead to the free secretion of tears, it is not surprising that
music should be apt to cause our eyes to become suffused with tears, especially when we are already softened
by any of the tenderer feelings. Music often produces another peculiar effect. We know that every strong
sensation, emotion, or excitement extreme pain, rage, terror, joy, or the passion of love all have a
special tendency to cause the muscles to tremble; and the thrill or slight shiver which runs down the backbone
and limbs of many persons when they are powerfully affected by music, seems to bear the same relation to
the above trembling of the body, as a slight suffusion of tears from the power of music does to weeping from
any strong and real emotion.
_Devotion_.As devotion is, in some degree, related to affection, though mainly consisting of reverence,
often combined with fear, the expression of this state of mind may here be briefly noticed. With some sects,
both past and present, religion and love have been strangely combined; and it has even been maintained,
lamentable as the fact may be, that the holy kiss of love differs but little from that which a man bestows on a
woman, or a woman on a man.[25] Devotion is chiefly expressed by the face being directed towards the
heavens, with the eyeballs upturned. Sir C. Bell remarks that, at the approach of sleep, or of a faintingfit, or
of death, the pupils are drawn upwards and inwards; and he believes that "when we are wrapt in devotional
feelings, and outward impressions are unheeded, the eyes are raised by an action neither taught nor acquired."
and that this is due to the same cause as in the above cases.[26] That the eyes are upturned during sleep is, as
I hear from Professor Donders, certain. With babies, whilst sucking their mother's breast, this movement of
the eyeballs often gives to them an absurd appearance of ecstatic delight; and here it may be clearly perceived
that a struggle is going on against the position naturally assumed during sleep. But Sir C. Bell's explanation
of the fact, which rests on the assumption that certain muscles are more under the control of the will than
others is, as I hear from Professor Donders, incorrect. As the eyes are often turned up in prayer, without the
mind being so much absorbed in thought as to approach to the unconsciousness of sleep, the movement is
probably a conventional one the result of the common belief that Heaven, the source of Divine power to
which we pray, is seated above us.
[24] `The Descent of Man,' vol. ii. p. 336.
A humble kneeling posture, with the hands upturned and palms joined, appears to us, from long habit, a
gesture so appropriate to devotion, that it might be thought to be innate; but I have not met with any evidence
to this effect with the various extraEuropean races of mankind. During the classical period of Roman history
it does not appear, as I hear from an excellent classic, that the hands were thus joined during prayer. Mr.
Rensleigh Wedgwood has apparently given[27] the true explanation, though this implies that the attitude is
one of slavish subjection. "When the suppliant kneels and holds up his hands with the palms joined, he
represents a captive who proves the completeness of his submission by offering up his hands to be bound by
the victor. It is the pictorial representation of the Latin _dare manus_, to signify submission." Hence it is not
probable that either the uplifting of the eyes or the joining of the open hands, under the influence of
devotional feelings, are innate or truly expressive actions; and this could hardly have been expected, for it is
very doubtful whether feelings, such as we should now rank as devotional, affected the hearts of men, whilst
they remained during past ages in an uncivilized condition.
[25] Dr. Mandsley has a discussion to this effect in his `Body and Mind,' 1870, p. 85.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION. 84
Page No 87
[26] `The Anatomy of Expression,' p. 103, and `Philosophical Transactions,' 1823, p. 182.
[27] `The Origin of Language,' 1866, p. 146. Mr. Tylor (`Early History of Mankind,' 2nd edit. 1870, p. 48)
gives a more complex origin to the position of the hands during prayer.
CHAPTER IX.
REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION.
The act of frowningReflection with an effort, or with the perception of something difficult or
disagreeable Abstracted meditationIlltemperMorosenessObstinacy Sulkiness and
poutingDecision or determinationThe firm closure of the mouth.
THE corrugators, by their contraction, lower the eyebrows and bring them together, producing vertical
furrows on the foreheadthat is, a frown. Sir C. Bell, who erroneously thought that the corrugator was
peculiar to man, ranks it as "the most remarkable muscle of the human face. It knits the eyebrows with an
energetic effort, which unaccountably, but irresistibly, conveys the idea of mind." Or, as he elsewhere says,
"when the eyebrows are knit, energy of mind is apparent, and there is the mingling of thought and emotion
with the savage and brutal rage of the mere animal."[1] There is much truth in these remarks, but hardly the
whole truth. Dr. Duchenne has called the corrugator the muscle of reflection;[2] but this name, without some
limitation, cannot be considered as quite correct.
[1] `Anatomy of Expression,' pp. 137, 139. It is not surprising that the corrugators should have become much
more developed in man than in the anthropoid apes; for they are brought into incessant action by him under
various circumstances, and will have been strengthened and modified by the inherited effects of use. We have
seen how important a part they play, together with the orbiculares, in protecting the eyes from being too
much gorged with blood during violent expiratory movements. When the eyes are closed as quickly and as
forcibly as possible, to save them from being injured by a blow, the corrugators contract. With savages or
other men whose heads are uncovered, the eyebrows are continually lowered and contracted to serve as a
shade against a too strong light; and this is effected partly by the corrugators. This movement would have
been more especially serviceable to man, as soon as his early progenitors held their heads erect. Lastly, Prof.
Donders believes (`Archives of Medicine,' ed. by L. Beale, 1870, vol. v. p. 34), that the corrugators are
brought into action in causing the eyeball to advance in accommodation for proximity in vision.
A man may be absorbed in the deepest thought, and his brow will remain smooth until he encounters some
obstacle in his train of reasoning, or is interrupted by some disturbance, and then a frown passes like a
shadow over his brow. A halfstarved man may think intently how to obtain food, but he probably will not
frown unless he encounters either in thought or action some difficulty, or finds the food when obtained
nauseous. I have noticed that almost everyone instantly frowns if he perceives a strange or bad taste in what
he is eating. I asked several persons, without explaining my object, to listen intently to a very gentle tapping
sound, the nature and source of which they all perfectly knew, and not one frowned; but a man who joined us,
and who could not conceive what we were all doing in profound silence, when asked to listen, frowned much,
though not in an illtemper, and said he could not in the least understand what we all wanted. Dr. Piderit[3]
who has published remarks to the same effect, adds that stammerers generally frown in speaking, and that a
man in doing even so trifling a thing as pulling on a boot, frowns if he finds it too tight. Some persons are
such habitual frowners, that the mere effort of speaking almost always causes their brows to contract.
[2] `Mecanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,' Album, Legende iii.
[3] `Mimik und Physiognomik,' s. 46.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IX. REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION. 85
Page No 88
Men of all races frown when they are in any way perplexed in thought, as I infer from the answers which I
have received to my queries; but I framed them badly, confounding absorbed meditation with perplexed
reflection. Nevertheless, it is clear that the Australians, Malays, Hindoos, and Kafirs of South Africa frown,
when they are puzzled. Dobritzhoffer remarks that the Guaranies of South America on like occasions knit
their brows.[4]
From these considerations, we may conclude that frowning is not the expression of simple reflection,
however profound, or of attention, however close, but of something difficult or displeasing encountered in a
train of thought or in action. Deep reflection can, however, seldom be long carried on without some
difficulty, so that it will generally be accompanied by a frown. Hence it is that frowning commonly gives to
the countenance, as Sir C. Bell remarks, an aspect of intellectual energy. But in order that this effect may be
produced, the eyes must be clear and steady, or they may be cast downwards, as often occurs in deep thought.
The countenance must not be otherwise disturbed, as in the case of an illtempered or peevish man, or of one
who shows the effects of prolonged suffering, with dulled eyes and drooping jaw, or who perceives a bad
taste in his food, or who finds it difficult to perform some trifling act, such as threading a needle. In these
cases a frown may often be seen, but it will be accompanied by some other expression, which will entirely
prevent the countenance having an appearance of intellectual energy or of profound thought.
[4] `History of the Abipones,' Eng. translat. vol. ii. p. 59, as quoted by Lubbock, `Origin of Civilisation,'
1870, p. 355.
We may now inquire how it is that a frown should express the perception of something difficult or
disagreeable, either in thought or action. In the same way as naturalists find it advisable to trace the
embryological development of an organ in order fully to understand its structure, so with the movements of
expression it is advisable to follow as nearly as possible the same plan. The earliest and almost sole
expression seen during the first days of infancy, and then often exhibited is that displayed during the act of
screaming; and screaming is excited, both at first and for some time afterwards, by every distressing or
displeasing sensation and emotion,by hunger, pain, anger, jealousy, fear, At such times the muscles round
the eyes are strongly contracted; and this, as I believe, explains to a large extent the act of frowning during
the remainder of our lives. I repeatedly observed my own infants, from under the age of one week to that of
two or three months, and found that when a screamingfit came on gradually, the first sign was the
contraction of the corrugators, which produced a slight frown, quickly followed by the contraction of the
other muscles round the eyes. When an infant is uncomfortable or unwell, little frownsas I record in my
notesmay be seen incessantly passing like shadows over its face; these being generally, but not always,
followed sooner or later by a cryingfit. For instance, I watched for some time a baby, between seven and
eight weeks old, sucking some milk which was cold, and therefore displeasing to him; and a steady little
frown was maintained all the time. This was never developed into an actual cryingfit, though occasionally
every stage of close approach could be observed.
As the habit of contracting the brows has been followed by infants during innumerable generations, at the
commencement of every crying or screaming fit, it has become firmly associated with the incipient sense of
something distressing or disagreeable. Hence under similar circumstances it would be apt to be continued
during maturity, although never then developed into a cryingfit. Screaming or weeping begins to be
voluntarily restrained at an early period of life, whereas frowning is hardly ever restrained at any age. It is
perhaps worth notice that with children much given to weeping, anything which perplexes their minds, and
which would cause most other children merely to frown, readily makes them weep. So with certain classes of
the insane, any effort of mind, however slight, which with an habitual frowner would cause a slight frown,
leads to their weeping in an unrestrained manner. It is not more surprising that the habit of contracting the
brows at the first perception of something distressing, although gained during infancy, should be retained
during the rest of our lives, than that many other associated habits acquired at an early age should be
permanently retained both by man and the lower animals. For instance, fullgrown cats, when feeling warm
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IX. REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION. 86
Page No 89
and comfortable, often retain the habit of alternately protruding their forefeet with extended toes, which
habit they practised for a definite purpose whilst sucking their mothers.
Another and distinct cause has probably strengthened the habit of frowning, whenever the mind is intent on
any subject and encounters some difficulty. Vision is the most important of all the senses, and during
primeval times the closest attention must have been incessantly: directed towards distant objects for the sake
of obtaining prey and avoiding danger. I remember being struck, whilst travelling in parts of South America,
which were dangerous from the presence of Indians, how incessantly, yet as it appeared unconsciously, the
halfwild Gauchos closely scanned the whole horizon. Now, when any one with no covering on his head (as
must have been aboriginally the case with mankind), strives to the utmost to distinguish in broad daylight,
and especially if the sky is bright, a distant object, he almost invariably contracts his brows to prevent the
entrance of too much light; the lower eyelids, cheeks, and upper lip being at the same time raised, so as to
lessen the orifice of the eyes. I have purposely asked several persons, young and old, to look, under the above
circumstances, at distant objects, making them believe that I only wished to test the power of their vision; and
they all behaved in the manner just described. Some of them, also, put their open, flat hands over their eyes to
keep out the excess of light. Gratiolet, after making some remarks to nearly the same effect,[5] says, "Ce sont
la des attitudes de vision difficile." He concludes that the muscles round the eyes contract partly for the sake
of excluding too much light (which appears to me the more important end), and partly to prevent all rays
striking the retina, except those which come direct from the object that is scrutinized. Mr. Bowman, whom I
consulted on this point, thinks that the contraction of the surrounding muscles may, in addition, "partly
sustain the consensual movements of the two eyes, by giving a firmer support while the globes are brought to
binocular vision by their own proper muscles."
As the effort of viewing with care under a bright light a distant object is both difficult and irksome, and as
this effort has been habitually accompanied, during numberless generations, by the contraction of the
eyebrows, the habit of frowning will thus have been much strengthened; although it was originally practised
during infancy from a quite independent cause, namely as the first step in the protection of the eyes during
screaming. There is, indeed, much analogy, as far as the state of the mind is concerned, between intently
scrutinizing a distant object, and following out an obscure train of thought, or performing some little and
troublesome mechanical work. The belief that the habit of contracting the brows is continued when there is
no need whatever to exclude too much light, receives support from the cases formerly alluded to, in which the
eyebrows or eyelids are acted on under certain circumstances in a useless manner, from having been similarly
used, under analogous circumstances, for a serviceable purpose. For instance, we voluntarily close our eyes
when we do not wish to see any object, and we are apt to close them, when we reject a proposition, as if we
could not or would not see it; or when we think about something horrible. We raise our eyebrows when we
wish to see quickly all round us, and we often do the same, when we earnestly desire to remember something;
acting as if we endeavoured to see it.
[5] `De la Physionomie,' pp. 15, 144, 146. Mr. Herbert Spencer accounts for frowning exclusively by the
habit of contracting the brows as a shade to the eyes in a bright light: see `Principles of Physiology,' 2nd edit.
1872, p. 546.
_Abstraction. Meditation_.When a person is lost in thought with his mind absent, or, as it is sometimes
said, "when he is in a brown study," he does not frown, but his eyes appear vacant. The lower eyelids are
generally raised and wrinkled, in the same manner as when a shortsighted person tries to distinguish a
distant object; and the upper orbicular muscles are at the same time slightly contracted. The wrinkling of the
lower eyelids under these circumstances has been observed with some savages, as by Mr. Dyson Lacy with
the Australians of Queensland, and several times by Mr. Geach with the Malays of the interior of Malacca.
What the meaning or cause of this action may be, cannot at present be explained; but here we have another
instance of movement round the eyes in relation to the state of the mind.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IX. REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION. 87
Page No 90
The vacant expression of the eyes is very peculiar, and at once shows when a man is completely lost in
thought. Professor Donders has, with his usual kindness, investigated this subject for me. He has observed
others in this condition, and has been himself observed by Professor Engelmann. The eyes are not then fixed
on any object, and therefore not, as I had imagined, on some distant object. The lines of vision of the two
eyes even often become slightly divergent; the divergence, if the head be held vertically, with the plane of
vision horizontal, amounting to an angle of 2'0 as a maximum. This was ascertained by observing the crossed
double image of a distant object. When the head droops forward, as often occurs with a man absorbed in
thought, owing to the general relaxation of his muscles, if the plane of vision be still horizontal, the eyes are
necessarily a little turned upwards, and then the divergence is as much as 3'0, or 3'0 5': if the eyes are turned
still more upwards, it amounts to between 6'0 and 7'0. Professor Donders attributes this divergence to the
almost complete relaxation of certain muscles of the eyes, which would be apt to follow from the mind being
wholly absorbed.[6] The active condition of the muscles of the eyes is that of convergence; and Professor
Donders remarks, as bearing on their divergence during a period of complete abstraction, that when one eye
becomes blind, it almost always, after a short lapse of time, deviates outwards; for its muscles are no longer
used in moving the eyeball inwards for the sake of binocular vision.
[6] Gratiolet remarks (De la Phys. p. 35), "Quand l'attention est fixee sur quelque image interieure, l'oeil
regarde dqns le vide et s'associe automatiquement a la contemplation de l'esprit." But this view hardly
deserves to be called an explanation.
Perplexed reflection is often accompanied by certain movements or gestures. At such times we commonly
raise our hands to our foreheads, mouths, or chins; but we do not act thus, as far as I have seen, when we are
quite lost in meditation, and no difficulty is encountered. Plautus, describing in one of his plays[7] a puzzled
man, says, "Now look, he has pillared his chin upon his hand." Even so trifling and apparently unmeaning a
gesture as the raising of the hand to the face has been observed with some savages. Al. J. Mansel Weale has
seen it with the Kafirs of South Africa; and the native chief Gaika adds, that men then "sometimes pull their
beards." Mr. Washington Matthews, who attended to some of the wildest tribes of Indians in the western
regions of the United States, remarks that he has seen them when concentrating their thoughts, bring their
"hands, usually the thumb and index finger, in contact with some part of the face, commonly the upper lip."
We can understand why the forehead should be pressed or rubbed, as deep thought tries the brain; but why
the hand should be raised to the mouth or face is far from clear.
_Illtemper_.We have seen that frowning is the natural expression of some difficulty encountered, or of
something disagreeable experienced either in thought or action, and he whose mind is often and readily
affected in this way, will be apt to be illtempered, or slightly angry, or peevish, and will commonly show it
by frowning. But a cross expression, due to a frown, may be counteracted, if the mouth appears sweet, from
being habitually drawn into a smile, and the eyes are bright and cheerful. So it will be if the eye is clear and
steady, and there is the appearance of earnest reflection. Frowning, with some depression of the corners of the
mouth, which is a sign of grief, gives an air of peevishness. If a child (see Plate IV., fig. 2)[8] frowns much
whilst crying, but does not strongly contract in the usual manner the orbicular muscles, a wellmarked
expression of anger or even of rage, together with misery, is displayed.
[7] `Miles Gloriosus,' act ii. sc. 2.
If the whole frowning brow be drawn much downward by the contraction of the pyramidal muscles of the
nose, which produces transverse wrinkles or folds across the base of the nose, the expression becomes one of
moroseness. Duchenne believes that the contraction of this muscle, without any frowning, gives the
appearance of extreme and aggressive hardness.[9] But I much doubt whether this is a true or natural
expression. I have shown Duchenne's photograph of a young man, with this muscle strongly contracted by
means of galvanism, to eleven persons, including some artists, and none of them could form an idea what was
intended, except one, a girl, who answered correctly, "surely reserve." When I first looked at this photograph,
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IX. REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION. 88
Page No 91
knowing what was intended, my imagination added, as I believe, what was necessary, namely, a frowning
brow; and consequently the expression appeared to me true and extremely morose.
[8] The original photograph by Herr Kindermann is much more expressive than this copy, as it shows the
frown on the brow more plainly.
[9] `Mecanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,' Album, Legende iv. figs. 1618.
A firmly closed mouth, in addition to a lowered and frowning brow, gives determination to the expression, or
may make it obstinate and sullen. How it comes that the firm closure of the mouth gives the appearance of
determination will presently be discussed. An expression of sullen obstinacy has been clearly recognized by
my informants, in the natives of six different regions of Australia. It is well marked, according to Mr. Scott,
with the Hindoos. It has been recognized with the Malays, Chinese, Kafirs, Abyssinians, and in a conspicuous
degree, according to Dr. Rothrock, with the wild Indians of North America, and according to Mr. D. Forbes,
with the Aymaras of Bolivia. I have also observed it with the Araucanos of southern Chili. Mr. Dyson Lacy
remarks that the natives of Australia, when in this frame of mind, sometimes fold their arms across their
breasts, an attitude which may be seen with us. A firm determination, amounting to obstinacy, is, also,
sometimes expressed by both shoulders being kept raised, the meaning of which gesture will be explained in
the following chapter.
With young children sulkiness is shown by pouting, or, as it is sometimes called, "making a snout."[10]
When the corners of the mouth are much depressed, the lower lip is a little everted and protruded; and this is
likewise called a pout. But the pouting here referred to, consists of the protrusion of both lips into a tubular
form, sometimes to such an extent as to project as far as the end of the nose, if this be short. Pouting is
generally accompanied by frowning, and sometimes by the utterance of a booing or whooing noise. This
expression is remarkable, as almost the sole one, as far as I know, which is exhibited much more plainly
during childhood, at least with Europeans, than during maturity. There is, however, some tendency to the
protrusion of the lips with the adults of all races under the influence of great rage. Some children pout when
they are shy, and they can then hardly be called sulky.
[10] Hensleigh Wedgwood on `The Origin of Language,' 1866, p. 78.
From inquiries which I have made in several large families, pouting does not seem very common with
European children; but it prevails throughout the world, and must be both common and strongly marked with
most savage races, as it has caught the attention of many observers. It has been noticed in eight different
districts of Australia; and one of my informants remarks how greatly the lips of the children are then
protruded. Two observers have seen pouting with the children of Hindoos; three, with those of the Kafirs and
Fingoes of South Africa, and with the Hottentots; and two, with the children of the wild Indians of North
America. Pouting has also been observed with the Chinese, Abyssinians, Malays of Malacca, Dyaks of
Borneo, and often with the New Zealanders. Mr. Mansel Weale informs me that he has seen the lips much
protruded, not only with the children of the Kafirs, but with the adults of both sexes when sulky; and Mr.
Stack has sometimes observed the same thing with the men, and very frequently with the women of New
Zealand. A trace of the same expression may occasionally be detected even with adult Europeans.
We thus see that the protrusion of the lips, especially with young children, is characteristic of sulkiness
throughout the greater part of the world. This movement apparently results from the retention, chiefly during
youth, of a primordial habit, or from an occasional reversion to it. Young orangs and chimpanzees protrude
their lips to an extraordinary degree, as described in a former chapter, when they are discontented, somewhat
angry, or sulky; also when they are surprised, a little frightened, and even when slightly pleased. Their
mouths are protruded apparently for the sake of making the various noises proper to these several states of
mind; and its shape, as I observed with the chimpanzee, differed slightly when the cry of pleasure and that of
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IX. REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION. 89
Page No 92
anger were uttered. As soon as these animals become enraged, the shape of the month wholly changes, and
the teeth are exposed. The adult orang when wounded is said to emit "a singular cry, consisting at first of high
notes, which at length deepen into a low roar. While giving out the high notes he thrusts out his lips into a
funnel shape, but in uttering the low notes he holds his mouth wide open."[11] With the gorilla, the lower lip
is said to be capable of great elongation. If then our semihuman progenitors protruded their lips when sulky
or a little angered, in the same manner as do the existing anthropoid apes, it is not an anomalous, though a
curious fact, that our children should exhibit, when similarly affected, a trace of the same expression, together
with some tendency to utter a noise. For it is not at all unusual for animals to retain, more or less perfectly,
during early youth, and subsequently to lose, characters which were aboriginally possessed by their adult
progenitors, and which are still retained by distinct species, their near relations.
Nor is it an anomalous fact that the children of savages should exhibit a stronger tendency to protrude their
lips, when sulky, than the children of civilized Europeans; for the essence of savagery seems to consist in the
retention of a primordial condition, and this occasionally holds good even with bodily peculiarities.[12] It
may be objected to this view of the origin of pouting, that the anthropoid apes likewise protrude their lips
when astonished and even when a little pleased; whilst with us this expression is generally confined to a
sulky frame of mind. But we shall see in a future chapter that with men of various races surprise does
sometimes lead to a slight protrusion of the lips, though great surprise or astonishment is more commonly
shown by the mouth being widely opened. As when we smile or laugh we draw back the corners of the
mouth, we have lost any tendency to protrude the lips, when pleased, if indeed our early progenitors thus
expressed pleasure.
[11] Muller, as quoted by Huxley, `Man's Place in Nature,' 1863, p. 38.
A little gesture made by sulky children may here be noticed, namely, their "showing a cold shoulder." This
has a different meaning, as, I believe, from the keeping both shoulders raised. A cross child, sitting on its
parent's knee, will lift up the near shoulder, then jerk it away, as if from a caress, and afterwards give a
backward push with it, as if to push away the offender. I have seen a child, standing at some distance from
any one, clearly express its feelings by raising one shoulder, giving it a little backward movement, and then
turning away its whole body.
_Decision or determination_.The firm closure of the mouth tends to give an expression of determination or
decision to the countenance. No determined man probably ever had an habitually gaping mouth. Hence, also,
a small and weak lower jaw, which seems to indicate that the mouth is not habitually and firmly closed, is
commonly thought to be characteristic of feebleness of character. A prolonged effort of any kind, whether of
body or mind, implies previous determination; and if it can be shown that the mouth is generally closed with
firmness before and during a great and continued exertion of the muscular system, then, through the principle
of association, the mouth would almost certainly be closed as soon as any determined resolution was taken.
Now several observers have noticed that a man, in commencing any violent muscular effort, invariably first
distends his lungs with air, and then compresses it by the strong contraction of the muscles of the chest; and
to effect this the mouth must be firmly closed. Moreover, as soon as the man is compelled to draw breath, he
still keeps his chest as much distended as possible.
[11] I have given several instances in my `Descent of Man,' vol. i. chap. iv.
Various causes have been assigned for this manner of acting. Sir C. Bell maintains[13] that the chest is
distended with air, and is kept distended at such times, in order to give a fixed support to the muscles which
are thereto attached. Hence, as he remarks, when two men are engaged in a deadly contest, a terrible silence
prevails, broken only by hard stifled breathing. There is silence, because to expel the air in the utterance of
any sound would be to relax the support for the muscles of the arms. If an outcry is heard, supposing the
struggle to take place in the dark, we at once know that one of the two has given up in despair.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER IX. REFLECTIONMEDITATIONILLTEMPERSULKINESSDETERMINATION. 90
Page No 93
Gratiolet admits[14] that when a man has to struggle with another to his utmost, or has to support a great
weight, or to keep for a long time the same forced attitude, it is necessary for him first to make a deep
inspiration, and then to cease breathing; but he thinks that Sir C. Bell's explanation is erroneous. He maintains
that arrested respiration retards the circulation of the blood, of which I believe there is no doubt, and he
adduces some curious evidence from the structure of the lower animals, showing, on the one hand, that a
retarded circulation is necessary for prolonged muscular exertion, and, on the other hand, that a rapid
circulation is necessary for rapid movements. According to this view, when we commence any great exertion,
we close our mouths and stop breathing, in order to retard the circulation of the blood. Gratiolet sums up the
subject by saying, "C'est la la vraie theorie de l'effort continu;" but how far this theory is admitted by other
physiologists I do not know.
[13] `Anatomy of Expression.' p. 190.
[14] `De la Physionomie,' pp. 118121.
Dr. Piderit accounts[15] for the firm closure of the mouth during strong muscular exertion, on the principle
that the influence of the will spreads to other muscles besides those necessarily brought into action in making
any particular exertion; and it is natural that the muscles of respiration and of the mouth, from being so
habitually used, should be especially liable to be thus acted on. It appears to me that there probably is some
truth in this view, for we are apt to press the teeth hard together during violent exertion, and this is not
requisite to prevent expiration, whilst the muscles of the chest are strongly contracted.
Lastly, when a man has to perform some delicate and difficult operation, not requiring the exertion of any
strength, he nevertheless generally closes his mouth and ceases for a time to breathe; but he acts thus in order
that the movements of his chest may not disturb, those of his arms. A person, for instance, whilst threading a
needle, may be seen to compress his lips and either to stop breathing, or to breathe as quietly as possible. So
it was, as formerly stated, with a young and sick chimpanzee, whilst it amused itself by killing flies with its
knuckles, as they buzzed about on the windowpanes. To perform an action, however trifling, if difficult,
implies some amount of previous determination.
[15] `Mimik und Physiognomik,' s. 79.
There appears nothing improbable in all the above assigned causes having come into play in different
degrees, either conjointly or separately, on various occasions. The result would be a wellestablished habit,
now perhaps inherited, of firmly closing the mouth at the commencement of and during any violent and
prolonged exertion, or any delicate operation. Through the principle of association there would also be a
strong tendency towards this same habit, as soon as the mind had resolved on any particular action or line of
conduct, even before there was any bodily exertion, or if none were requisite. The habitual and firm closure
of the mouth would thus come to show decision of character; and decision readily passes into obstinacy.
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER.
HatredRage, effects of on the systemUncovering of the teeth Rage in the insaneAnger and
indignationAs expressed by the various races of manSneering and defianceThe uncovering of the
canine tooth on one side of the face.
IF we have suffered or expect to suffer some wilful injury from a man, or if he is in any way offensive to us,
we dislike him; and dislike easily rises into hatred. Such feelings, if experienced in a moderate degree, are not
clearly expressed by any movement of the body or features, excepting perhaps by a certain gravity of
behaviour, or by some illtemper. Few individuals, however, can long reflect about a hated person, without
feeling and exhibiting signs of indignation or rage. But if the offending person be quite insignificant, we
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. 91
Page No 94
experience merely disdain or contempt. If, on the other hand, he is allpowerful, then hatred passes into
terror, as when a slave thinks about a cruel master, or a savage about a bloodthirsty malignant deity.[1] Most
of our emotions are so closely connected with their expression, that they hardly exist if the body remains
passivethe nature of the expression depending in chief part on the nature of the actions which have been
habitually performed under this particular state of the mind. A man, for instance, may know that his life is in
the extremest peril, and may strongly desire to save if; yet, as Louis XVI. said, when surrounded by a fierce
mob, "Am I afraid? feel my pulse." So a man may intensely hate another, but until his bodily frame is
affected, he cannot be said to be enraged.
[1] See some remarks to this effect by Mr. Bain, `The Emotions and the Will,' 2nd edit. 1865, p. 127.
_Rage_.I have already had occasion to treat of this emotion in the third chapter, when discussing the direct
influence of the excited sensorium on the body, in combination with the effects of habitually associated
actions. Rage exhibits itself in the most diversified manner. The heart and circulation are always affected; the
face reddens or becomes purple, with the veins on the forehead and neck distended. The reddening of the skin
has been observed with the coppercoloured Indians of South America,[2] and even, as it is said, on the
white cicatrices left by old wounds on negroes.[3] Monkeys also redden from passion. With one of my own
infants, under four months old, I repeatedly observed that the first symptom of an approaching passion was
the rushing of the blood into his bare scalp. On the other hand, the action of the heart is sometimes so much
impeded by great rage, that the countenance becomes pallid or livid,[4] and not a few men with heartdisease
have dropped down dead under this powerful emotion.
[2] Rengger, Naturgesch. der Saugethiere von Paraguay, 1830, s. 3.
[3] Sir C. Bell, `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 96. On the other hand, Dr. Burgess (`Physiology of Blushing,'
1839, p. 31) speaks of the reddening of a cicatrix in a negress as of the nature of a blush.
[4] Moreau and Gratiolet have discussed the colour of the face under the influence of intense passion: see the
edit. of 1820 of Lavater, vol. iv. pp. 282 and 300; and Gratiolet, `De la Physionomie,' p. 345.
The respiration is likewise affected; the chest heaves, and the dilated nostrils quiver.[5] As Tennyson writes,
"sharp breaths of anger puffed her fairy nostrils out." Hence we have such expressions as breathing out
vengeance," and "fuming with anger."[6]
The excited brain gives strength to the muscles, and at the same time energy to the will. The body is
commonly held erect ready for instant action, but sometimes it is bent forward towards the offending person,
with the limbs more or less rigid. The mouth is generally closed with firmness, showing fixed determination,
and the teeth are clenched or ground together. Such gestures as the raising of the arms, with the fists
clenched, as if to strike the offender, are common. Few men in a great passion, and telling some one to
begone, can resist acting as if they intended to strike or push the man violently away. The desire, indeed, to
strike often becomes so intolerably strong, that inanimate objects are struck or dashed to the ground; but the
gestures frequently become altogether purposeless or frantic. Young children, when in a violent rage roll on
the ground on their backs or bellies, screaming, kicking, scratching, or biting everything within reach. So it
is, as I hear from Mr. Scott, with Hindoo children; and, as we have seen, with the young of the
anthropomorphous apes.
[6] Sir C. Bell `Anatomy of Expression,' pp. 91, 107) has fully discussed this subject. Moreau remarks (in the
edit. of 1820 of `La Physionomie, par G. Lavater,' vol. iv. p. 237), and quotes Portal in confirmation, that
asthmatic patients acquire permanently expanded nostrils, owing to the habitual contraction of the elevatory
muscles of the wings of the nose. The explanation by Dr. Piderit (`Mimik und Physiognomik,' s. 82) of the
distension of the nostrils, namely, to allow free breathing whilst the mouth is closed and the teeth clenched,
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. 92
Page No 95
does not appear to be nearly so correct as that by Sir C. Bell, who attributes it to the sympathy (_i. e_.
habitual coaction) of all the respiratory muscles. The nostrils of an angry man may be seen to become
dilated, although his mouth is open.
[7] Mr. Wedgwood, `On the Origin of Language,' 1866, p. 76. He also observes that the sound of hard
breathing "is represented by the syllables _puff, huff, whiff_, whence a _huff_ is a fit of illtemper."
But the muscular system is often affected in a wholly different way; for trembling is a frequent consequence
of extreme rage. The paralysed lips then refuse to obey the will, "and the voice sticks in the throat;"[7] or it is
rendered loud, harsh, and discordant. If there be much and rapid speaking, the mouth froths. The hair
sometimes bristles; but I shall return to this subject in another chapter, when I treat of the mingled emotions
of rage and terror. There is in most cases a stronglymarked frown on the forehead; for this follows from the
sense of anything displeasing or difficult, together with concentration of mind. But sometimes the brow,
instead of being much contracted and lowered, remains smooth, with the glaring eyes kept widely open. The
eyes are always bright, or may, as Homer expresses it, glisten with fire. They are sometimes bloodshot, and
are said to protrude from their socketsthe result, no doubt, of the head being gorged with blood, as shown
by the veins being distended. According to Gratiolet," the pupils are always contracted in rage, and I hear
from Dr. Crichton Browne that this is the case in the fierce delirium of meningitis; but the movements of the
iris under the influence of the different emotions is a very obscure subject.
Shakspeare sums up the chief characteristics of rage as follows:
"In peace there's nothing so becomes a man,
As modest stillness and humility;
But when the blast of war blows in our ears,
Then imitate the action of the tiger:
Stiffen the sinews, summon up the blood,
Then lend the eye a terrible aspect;
Now set the teeth, and stretch the nostril wide,
Hold hard the breath, and bend up every spirit
To his full height! On, on, you noblest English."
_Henry V_., act iii. sc. 1.
[7] Sir C. Bell `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 95) has some excellent remarks on the expression of rage.
[8] `De la Physionomie,' 1865, p. 346.
The lips are sometimes protruded during rage in a manner, the meaning of which I do not understand, unless
it depends on our descent from some apelike animal. Instances have been observed, not only with
Europeans, but with the Australians and Hindoos. The lips, however, are much more commonly retracted, the
grinning or clenched teeth being thus exposed. This has been noticed by almost every one who has written on
expression.[9] The appearance is as if the teeth were uncovered, ready for seizing or tearing an enemy,
though there may be no intention of acting in this manner. Mr. Dyson Lacy has seen this grinning expression
with the Australians, when quarrelling, and so has Gaika with the Kafirs of South America. Dickens,[10] in
speaking of an atrocious murderer who had just been caught, and was surrounded by a furious mob, describes
"the people as jumping up one behind another, snarling with their teeth, and making at him like wild beasts."
Every one who has had much to do with young children must have seen how naturally they take to biting,
when in a passion. It seems as instinctive in them as in young crocodiles, who snap their little jaws as soon as
they emerge from the egg.
[9] Sir C. Bell, `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 177. Gratiolet (De la Phys. p. 369) says, `les dents se decouvrent,
et imitent symboliquement l'action de dechirer et de mordre.'I If, instead of using the vague term
_symboliquement_, Gratiolet had said that the action was a remnant of a habit acquired during primeval times
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. 93
Page No 96
when our semihuman progenitors fought together with their teeth, like gorillas and orangs at the present
day, he would have been more intelligible. Dr. Piderit (`Mimik,' s. 82) also speaks of the retraction of the
upper lip during rage. In an engraving of one of Hogarth's wonderful pictures, passion is represented in the
plainest manner by the open glaring eyes, frowning forehead, and exposed grinning teeth.
[10] `Oliver Twist,' vol. iii. p. 245.
A grinning expression and the protrusion of the lips appear sometimes to go together. A close observer says
that he has seen many instances of intense hatred (which can hardly be distinguished from rage, more or less
suppressed) in Orientals, and once in an elderly English woman. In all these cases there "was a grin, not a
scowlthe lips lengthening, the cheeks settling downwards, the eyes halfclosed, whilst the brow remained
perfectly calm."[11]
This retraction of the lips and uncovering of the teeth during paroxysms of rage, as if to bite the offender, is
so remarkable, considering how seldom the teeth are used by men in fighting, that I inquired from Dr. J.
Crichton Browne whether the habit was common in the insane whose passions are unbridled. He informs me
that he has repeatedly observed it both with the insane and idiotic, and has given me the following
illustrations:
Shortly before receiving my letter, be witnessed an uncontrollable outbreak of anger and delusive jealousy in
an insane lady. At first she vituperated her husband, and whilst doing so foamed at the mouth. Next she
approached close to him with compressed lips, and a virulent set frown. Then she drew back her lips,
especially the corners of the upper lip, and showed her teeth, at the same time aiming a vicious blow at him.
A second case is that of an old soldier, who, when he is requested to conform to the rules of the
establishment, gives way to discontent, terminating in fury. He commonly begins by asking Dr. Browne
whether he is not ashamed to treat him in such a manner. He then swears and blasphemes, paces tip and
down, tosses his arms wildly about, and menaces any one near him. At last, as his exasperation culminates, he
rushes up towards Dr. Browne with a peculiar sidelong movement, shaking his doubled fist, and threatening
destruction. Then his upper lip may be seen to be raised, especially at the corners, so that his huge canine
teeth are exhibited. He hisses forth his curses through his set teeth, and his whole expression assumes the
character of extreme ferocity. A similar description is applicable to another man, excepting that he generally
foams at the mouth and spits, dancing and jumping about in a strange rapid manner, shrieking out his
maledictions in a shrill falsetto voice.
[11] `The Spectator,' July 11, 1868, p. 810.
Dr. Browne also informs me of the case of an epileptic idiot, incapable of independent movements, and who
spends the whole day in playing with some toys; but his temper is morose and easily roused into fierceness.
When any one touches his toys, he slowly raises his head from its habitual downward position, and fixes his
eyes on the offender, with a tardy yet angry scowl. If the annoyance be repeated, he draws back his thick lips
and reveals a prominent row of hideous fangs (large canines being especially noticeable), and then makes a
quick and cruel clutch with his open hand at the offending person. The rapidity of this clutch, as Dr. Browne
remarks, is marvellous in a being ordinarily so torpid that he takes about fifteen seconds, when attracted by
any noise, to turn his head from one side to the other. If, when thus incensed, a handkerchief, book, or other
article, be placed into his hands, he drags it to his mouth and bites it. Mr. Nicol has likewise described to me
two cases of insane patients, whose lips are retracted during paroxysms of rage.
Dr. Maudsley, after detailing various strange animallike traits in idiots, asks whether these are not due to the
reappearance of primitive instincts"a faint echo from a fardistant past, testifying to a kinship which man
has almost outgrown." He adds, that as every human brain passes, in the course of its development, through
the same stages as those occurring in the lower vertebrate animals, and as the brain of an idiot is in an
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. 94
Page No 97
arrested condition, we may presume that it "will manifest its most primitive functions, and no higher
functions." Dr. Maudsley thinks that the same view may be extended to the brain in its degenerated condition
in some insane patients; and asks, whence come "the savage snarl, the destructive disposition, the obscene
language, the wild howl, the offensive habits, displayed by some of the insane? Why should a human being,
deprived of his reason, ever become so brutal in character, as some do, unless he has the brute nature within
him?"[12] This question must, as it would appear, he answered in the affirmative.
_Anger, Indignation_.These states of the mind differ from rage only in degree, and there is no marked
distinction in their characteristic signs. Under moderate anger the action of the heart is a little increased, the
colour heightened, and the eyes become bright. The respiration is likewise a little hurried; and as all the
muscles serving for this function act in association, the wings of the nostrils are somewhat raised to allow of
a free indraught of air; and this is a highly characteristic sign of indignation. The mouth is commonly
compressed, and there is almost always a frown on the brow. Instead of the frantic gestures of extreme rage,
an indignant man unconsciously throws himself into an attitude ready for attacking or striking his enemy,
whom he will perhaps scan from head to foot in defiance. He carries his head erect, with his chest well
expanded, and the feet planted firmly on the ground. He holds his arms in various positions, with one or both
elbows squared, or with the arms rigidly suspended by his sides. With Europeans the fists are commonly
clenched.[13] The figures 1 and 2 in Plate VI. are fairly good representations of men simulating indignation.
Any one may see in a mirror, if he will vividly imagine that he has been insulted and demands an explanation
in an angry tone of voice, that he suddenly and unconsciously throws himself into some such attitude.
[12] `Body and Mind,' 1870, pp. 5153.
Rage, anger, and indignation are exhibited in nearly the same manner throughout the world; and the
following descriptions may be worth giving as evidence of this, and as illustrations of some of the foregoing
remarks. There is, however, an exception with respect to clenching the fists, which seems confined chiefly to
the men who fight with their fists. With the Australians only one of my informants has seen the fists
clenched. All agree about the body being held erect; and all, with two exceptions, state that the brows are
heavily contracted. Some of them allude to the firmlycompressed mouth, the distended nostrils, and flashing
eyes. According to the Rev. Mr. Taplin, rage, with the Australians, is expressed by the lips being protruded,
the eyes being widely open; and in the case of the women by their dancing about and casting dust into the air.
Another observer speaks of the native men, when enraged, throwing their arms wildly about.
[13] Le Brun, in his wellknown `Conference sur l'Expression' (`La Physionomie, par Lavater,' edit. of 1820,
vol. lx. p. 268), remarks that anger is expressed by the clenching of the fists. See, to the same effect,
Huschke, `Mimices et Physiognomices, Fragmentum Physiologicum,' 1824, p. 20. Also Sir C. Bell,
`Anatomy of Expression,' p. 219.
I have received similar accounts, except as to the clenching of the fists, in regard to the Malays of the
Malacca peninsula, the Abyssinians, and the natives of South Africa. So it is with the Dakota Indians of
North America; and, according to Mr. Matthews, they then hold their heads erect, frown, and often stalk
away with long strides. Mr. Bridges states that the Fuegians, when enraged, frequently stamp on the ground,
walk distractedly about, sometimes cry and grow pale. The Rev. Mr. Stack watched a New Zealand man and
woman quarrelling, and made the following entry in his notebook: "Eyes dilated, body swayed violently
backwards and forwards, head inclined forwards, fists clenched, now thrown behind the body, now directed
towards each other's faces." Mr. Swinhoe says that my description agrees with what he has seen of the
Chinese, excepting that an angry man generally inclines his body towards his antagonist, and pointing at him,
pours forth a volley of abuse.
Lastly, with respect to the natives of India, Mr. J. Scott has sent me a full description of their gestures and
expression when enraged. Two lowcaste Bengalees disputed about a loan. At first they were calm, but soon
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. 95
Page No 98
grew furious and poured forth the grossest abuse on each other's relations and progenitors for many
generations past. Their gestures were very different from those of Europeans; for though their chests were
expanded and shoulders squared, their arms remained rigidly suspended, with the elbows turned inwards and
the hands alternately clenched and opened. Their shoulders were often raised high, and then again lowered.
They looked fiercely at each other from under their lowered and strongly wrinkled brows, and their protruded
lips were firmly closed. They approached each other, with heads and necks stretched forwards, and pushed,
scratched, and grasped at each other. This protrusion of the head and body seems a common gesture with the
enraged; and I have noticed it with degraded English women whilst quarrelling violently in the streets. In
such cases it may be presumed that neither party expects to receive a blow from the other.
A Bengalee employed in the Botanic Gardens was accused, in the presence of Mr. Scott, by the native
overseer of having stolen a valuable plant. He listened silently and scornfully to the accusation; his attitude
erect, chest expanded, mouth closed, lips protruding, eyes firmly set and penetrating. He then defiantly
maintained his innocence, with upraised and clenched hands, his head being now pushed forwards, with the
eyes widely open and eyebrows raised. Mr. Scott also watched two Mechis, in Sikhim, quarrelling about their
share of payment. They soon got into a furious passion, and then their bodies became less erect, with their
heads pushed forwards; they made grimaces at each other; their shoulders were raised; their arms rigidly bent
inwards at the elbows, and their hands spasmodically closed, but not properly clenched. They continually
approached and retreated from each other, and often raised their arms as if to strike, but their hands were
open, and no blow was given. Mr. Scott made similar observations on the Lepchas whom he often saw
quarrelling, and he noticed that they kept their arms rigid and almost parallel to their bodies, with the hands
pushed somewhat backwards and partially closed, but not clenched.
_Sneering, Defiance: Uncovering the canine tooth on one side_. The expression which I wish here to
consider differs but little from that already described, when the lips are retracted and the grinning teeth
exposed. The difference consists solely in the upper lip being retracted in such a manner that the canine tooth
on one side of the face alone is shown; the face itself being generally a little upturned and half averted from
the person causing offence. The other signs of rage are not necessarily present. This expression may
occasionally be observed in a person who sneers at or defies another, though there may be no real anger; as
when any one is playfully accused of some fault, and answers, "I scorn the imputation." The expression is not
a common one, but I have seen it exhibited with perfect distinctness by a lady who was being quizzed by
another person. It was described by Parsons as long ago as 1746, with an engraving, showing the uncovered
canine on one side.[14] Mr. Rejlander, without my having made any allusion to the subject, asked me
whether I had ever noticed this expression, as he had been much struck by it. He has photographed for me
(Plate IV. fig 1) a lady, who sometimes unintentionally displays the canine on one side, and who can do so
voluntarily with unusual distinctness.
The expression of a halfplayful sneer graduates into one of great ferocity when, together with a heavily
frowning brow and fierce eye, the canine tooth is exposed. A Bengalee boy was accused before Mr. Scott of
some misdeed. The delinquent did not dare to give vent to his wrath in words, but it was plainly shown on his
countenance, sometimes by a defiant frown, and sometimes "by a thoroughly canine snarl." When this was
exhibited, "the corner of the lip over the eyetooth, which happened in this case to be large and projecting,
was raised on the side of his accuser, a strong frown being still retained on the brow." Sir C. Bell states[15]
that the actor Cooke could express the most determined hate "when with the oblique cast of his eyes he drew
up the outer part of the upper lip, and discovered a sharp angular tooth."
[14] Transact. Philosoph. Soc., Appendix, 1746, p. 65.
The uncovering of the canine tooth is the result of a double movement. The angle or corner of the mouth is
drawn a little backwards, and at the same time a muscle which runs parallel to and near the nose draws up the
outer part of the upper lip, and exposes the canine on this side of the face. The contraction of this muscle
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. 96
Page No 99
makes a distinct furrow on the cheek, and produces strong wrinkles under the eye, especially at its inner
corner. The action is the same as that of a snarling dog; and a dog when pretending to fight often draws up the
lip on one side alone, namely that facing his antagonist. Our word _sneer_ is in fact the same as _snarl_,
which was originally _snar_, the _l_ "being merely an element implying continuance of action."[16]
I suspect that we see a trace of this same expression in what is called a derisive or sardonic smile. The lips are
then kept joined or almost joined, but one corner of the mouth is retracted on the side towards the derided
person; and this drawing back of the corner is part of a true sneer. Although some persons smile more on one
side of their face than on the other, it is not easy to understand why in cases of derision the smile, if a real
one, should so commonly be confined to one side. I have also on these occasions noticed a slight twitching of
the muscle which draws up the outer part of the upper lip; and this movement, if fully carried out, would have
uncovered the canine, and would have produced a true sneer.
[15] `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 136. Sir C. Bell calls (p. 131) the muscles which uncover the canines the
snarling muscles.
[16] Hensleigh Wedgwood, `Dictionary of English Etymology,' 1865, vol. iii. pp. 240, 243.
Mr. Bulmer, an Australian missionary in a remote part of Gipps' Land, says, in answer to my query about the
uncovering of the canine on one side, "I find that the natives in snarling at each other speak with the teeth
closed, the upper lip drawn to one side, and a general angry expression of face; but they look direct at the
person addressed." Three other observers in Australia, one in Abyssinia, and one in China, answer my query
on this head in the affirmative; but as the expression is rare, and as they enter into no details, I am afraid of
implicitly trusting them. It is, however, by no means improbable that this animallike expression may be
more common with savages than with civilized races. Mr. Geach is an observer who may be fully trusted, and
he has observed it on one occasion in a Malay in the interior of Malacca. The Rev. S. O. Glenie answers, "We
have observed this expression with the natives of Ceylon, but not often." Lastly, in North America, Dr.
Rothrock has seen it with some wild Indians, and often in a tribe adjoining the Atnahs.
Although the upper lip is certainly sometimes raised on one side alone in sneering at or defying any one, I do
not know that this is always the case, for the face is commonly half averted, and the expression is often
momentary. The movement being confined to one side may not be an essential part of the expression, but
may depend on the proper muscles being incapable of movement excepting on one side. I asked four persons
to endeavour to act voluntarily in this manner; two could expose the canine only on the left side, one only on
the right side, and the fourth on neither side. Nevertheless it is by no means certain that these same persons, if
defying any one in earnest, would not unconsciously have uncovered their canine tooth on the side,
whichever it might be, towards the offender. For we have seen that some persons cannot voluntarily make
their eyebrows oblique, yet instantly act in this manner when affected by any real, although most trifling,
cause of distress. The power of voluntarily uncovering the canine on one side of the face being thus often
wholly lost, indicates that it is a rarely used and almost abortive action. It is indeed a surprising fact that man
should possess the power, or should exhibit any tendency to its use; for Mr. Sutton has never noticed a
snarling action in our nearest allies, namely, the monkeys in the Zoological Gardens, and he is positive that
the baboons, though furnished with great canines, never act thus, but uncover all their teeth when feeling
savage and ready for an attack. Whether the adult anthropomorphous apes, in the males of whom the canines
are much larger than in the females, uncover them when prepared to fight, is not known.
The expression here considered, whether that of a playful sneer or ferocious snarl, is one of the most curious
which occurs in man. It reveals his animal descent; for no one, even if rolling on the ground in a deadly
grapple with an enemy, and attempting to bite him, would try to use his canine teeth more than his other
teeth. We may readily believe from our affinity to the anthropomorphous apes that our male semihuman
progenitors possessed great canine teeth, and men are now occasionally born having them of unusually large
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER. 97
Page No 100
size, with interspaces in the opposite jaw for their reception.[17] We may further suspect, notwithstanding
that we have no support from analogy, that our semihuman progenitors uncovered their canine teeth when
prepared for battle, as we still do when feeling ferocious, or when merely sneering at or defying some one,
without any intention of making a real attack with our teeth.
[17] `The Descent of Man,' 1871, vol. L p. 126.
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE,
ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION.
Contempt, scorn and disdain, variously expressedDerisive smile Gestures expressive of
contemptDisgustGuilt, deceit, pride, Helplessness or impotencePatienceObstinacyShrugging
the shoulders common to most of the races of manSigns of affirmation and negation.
SCORN and disdain can hardly be distinguished from contempt, excepting that they imply a rather more
angry frame of mind. Nor can they be clearly distinguished from the feelings discussed in the last chapter
under the terms of sneering and defiance. Disgust is a sensation rather more distinct in its nature and refers to
something revolting, primarily in relation to the sense of taste, as actually perceived or vividly imagined; and
secondarily to anything which causes a similar feeling, through the sense of smell, touch, and even of
eyesight. Nevertheless, extreme contempt, or as it is often called loathing contempt, hardly differs from
disgust. These several conditions of the mind are, therefore, nearly related; and each of them may be
exhibited in many different ways. Some writers have insisted chiefly on one mode of expression, and others
on a different mode. From this circumstance M. Lemoine has argued[1] that their descriptions are not
trustworthy. But we shall immediately see that it is natural that the feelings which we have here to consider
should be expressed in many different ways, inasmuch as various habitual actions serve equally well, through
the principle of association, for their expression.
Scorn and disdain, as well as sneering and defiance, may be displayed by a slight uncovering of the canine
tooth on one side of the face; and this movement appears to graduate into one closely like a smile. Or the
smile or laugh may be real, although one of derision; and this implies that the offender is so insignificant that
he excites only amusement; but the amusement is generally a pretence. Gaika in his answers to my queries
remarks, that contempt is commonly shown by his countrymen, the Kafirs, by smiling; and the Rajah Brooke
makes the same observation with respect to the Dyaks of Borneo. As laughter is primarily the expression of
simple joy, very young children do not, I believe, ever laugh in derision.
The partial closure of the eyelids, as Duchenne[2] insists, or the turning away of the eyes or of the whole
body, are likewise highly expressive of disdain. These actions seem to declare that the despised person is not
worth looking at or is disagreeable to behold. The accompanying photograph (Plate V. fig. 1) by Mr.
Rejlander, shows this form of disdain. It represents a young lady, who is supposed to be tearing up the
photograph of a despised lover.
The most common method of expressing contempt is by movements about the nose, or round the mouth; but
the latter movements, when strongly pronounced, indicate disgust. The nose may be slightly turned up, which
apparently follows from the turning up of the upper lip; or the movement may be abbreviated into the mere
wrinkling of the nose. The nose is often slightly contracted, so as partly to close the passage;[3] and this is
commonly accompanied by a slight snort or expiration. All these actions are the same with those which we
employ when we perceive an offensive odour, and wish to exclude or expel it. In extreme cases, as Dr. Piderit
remarks,[4] we protrude and raise both lips, or the upper lip alone, so as to close the nostrils as by a valve, the
nose being thus turned up. We seem thus to say to the despised person that he smells offensively,[5] in nearly
the same manner as we express to him by halfclosing our eyelids, or turning away our faces, that he is not
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 98
Page No 101
worth looking at. It must not, however, be supposed that such ideas actually pass through the mind when we
exhibit our contempt; but as whenever we have perceived a disagreeable odour or seen a disagreeable sight,
actions of this kind have been performed, they have become habitual or fixed, and are now employed under
any analogous state of mind.
[1] `De In Physionomie et la Parole,' 1865, p. 89.
[2] `Physionomie Humaine,' Album, Legende viii. p. 35. Gratiolet also speaks (De la Phys. 1865, p. 52) of the
turning away of the eyes and body.
[3] Dr. W. Ogle, in an interesting paper on the Sense of Smell (`MedicoChirurgical Transactions,' vol. liii. p.
268), shows that when we wish to smell carefully, instead of taking one deep nasal inspiration, we draw in
the air by a succession of rapid short sniffs. If "the nostrils be watched during this process, it will be seen that,
so far from dilating, they actually contract at each sniff. The contraction does not include the whole anterior
opening, but only the posterior portion." He then explains the cause of this movement. When, on the other
hand, we wish to exclude any odour, the contraction, I presume, affects only the anterior part of the nostrils.
[4] `Mimik und Physiognomik,' ss. 84, 93. Gratiolet (ibid. p. 155) takes nearly the same view with Dr. Piderit
respecting the expression of contempt and disgust.
[5] Scorn implies a strong form of contempt; and one of the roots of the word `scorn' means, according to Mr.
Wedgwood (Dict. of English Etymology, vol. iii. p. 125), ordure or dirt. A person who is scorned is treated
like dirt.
Various odd little gestures likewise indicate contempt; for instance, _snapping one's fingers_. This, as Mr.
Taylor remarks,[6] "is not very intelligible as we generally see it; but when we notice that the same sign made
quite gently, as if rolling some tiny object away between the finger and thumb, or the sign of flipping it away
with the thumbnail and forefinger, are usual and wellunderstood deafanddumb gestures, denoting
anything tiny, insignificant, contemptible, it seems as though we had exaggerated and conventionalized a
perfectly natural action, so as to lose sight of its original meaning. There is a curious mention of this gesture
by Strabo." Mr. Washington Matthews informs me that, with the Dakota Indians of North America, contempt
is shown not only by movements of the face, such as those above described, but "conventionally, by the hand
being closed and held near the breast, then, as the forearm is suddenly extended, the hand is opened and the
fingers separated from each other. If the person at whose expense the sign is made is present, the hand is
moved towards him, and the head sometimes averted from him." This sudden extension and opening of the
hand perhaps indicates the dropping or throwing away a valueless object.
The term `disgust,' in its simplest sense, means something offensive to the taste. It is curious how readily this
feeling is excited by anything unusual in the appearance, odour, or nature of our food. In Tierra del Fuego a
native touched with his finger some cold preserved meat which I was eating at our bivouac, and plainly
showed utter disgust at its softness; whilst I felt utter disgust at my food being touched by a naked savage,
though his hands did not appear dirty. A smear of soup on a man's beard looks disgusting, though there is of
course nothing disgusting in the soup itself. I presume that this follows from the strong association in our
minds between the sight of food, however circumstanced, and the idea of eating it.
[6] `Early History of Mankind,' 2nd edit. 1870, p. 45.
As the sensation of disgust primarily arises in connection with the act of eating or tasting, it is natural that its
expression should consist chiefly in movements round the mouth. But as disgust also causes annoyance, it is
generally accompanied by a frown, and often by gestures as if to push away or to guard oneself against the
offensive object. In the two photographs (figs. 2 and 3, on Plate V.) Mr. Rejlander has simulated this
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 99
Page No 102
expression with some success. With respect to the face, moderate disgust is exhibited in various ways; by the
mouth being widely opened, as if to let an offensive morsel drop out; by spitting; by blowing out of the
protruded lips; or by a sound as of clearing the throat. Such guttural sounds are written _ach_ or _ugh_; and
their utterance is sometimes accompanied by a shudder, the arms being pressed close to the sides and the
shoulders raised in the same manner as when horror is experienced.[7] Extreme disgust is expressed by
movements round the month identical with those preparatory to the act of vomiting. The mouth is opened
widely, with the upper lip strongly retracted, which wrinkles the sides of the nose, and with the lower lip
protruded and everted as much as possible. This latter movement requires the contraction of the muscles
which draw downwards the corners of the mouth.[8]
[7] See, to this effect, Mr. Hensleigh Wedgwood's Introduction to the `Dictionary of English Etymology,' 2nd
edit. 1872, p. xxxvii.
It is remarkable how readily and instantly retching or actual vomiting is induced in some persons by the mere
idea of having partaken of any unusual food, as of an animal which is not commonly eaten; although there is
nothing in such food to cause the stomach to reject it. When vomiting results, as a reflex action, from some
real cause as from too rich food, or tainted meat, or from an emeticit does not ensue immediately, but
generally after a considerable interval of time. Therefore, to account for retching or vomiting being so
quickly and easily excited by a mere idea, the suspicion arises that our progenitors must formerly have had
the power (like that possessed by ruminants and some other animals) of voluntarily rejecting food which
disagreed with them, or which they thought would disagree with them; and now, though this power has been
lost, as far as the will is concerned, it is called into involuntary action, through the force of a formerly
wellestablished habit, whenever the mind revolts at the idea of having partaken of any kind of food, or at
anything disgusting. This suspicion receives support from the fact, of which I am assured by Mr. Sutton, that
the monkeys in the Zoological Gardens often vomit whilst in perfect health, which looks as if the act were
voluntary. We can see that as man is able to communicate by language to his children and others, the
knowledge of the kinds of food to be avoided, he would have little occasion to use the faculty of voluntary
rejection; so that this power would tend to be lost through disuse.
[8] Duchenne believes that in the eversion of the lower lip, the corners are drawn downwards by the
_depressores anguli oris_. Henle (Handbuch d. Anat. des Menschen, 1858, B. i. s. 151) concludes that this is
effected by the _musculus quadratus menti_.
As the sense of smell is so intimately connected with that of taste, it is not surprising that an excessively bad
odour should excite retching or vomiting in some persons, quite as readily as the thought of revolting food
does; and that, as a further consequence, a moderately offensive odour should cause the various expressive
movements of disgust. The tendency to retch from a fetid odour is immediately strengthened in a curious
manner by some degree of habit, though soon lost by longer familiarity with the cause of offence and by
voluntary restraint. For instance, I wished to clean the skeleton of a bird, which had not been sufficiently
macerated, and the smell made my servant and myself (we not having had much experience in such work)
retch so violently, that we were compelled to desist. During the previous days I had examined some other
skeletons, which smelt slightly; yet the odour did not in the least affect me, but, subsequently for several
days, whenever I handled these same skeletons, they made me retch.
From the answers received from my correspondents it appears that the various movements, which have now
been described as expressing contempt and disgust, prevail throughout a large part of the world. Dr.
Rothrock, for instance, answers with a decided affirmative with respect to certain wild Indian tribes of North
America. Crantz says that when a Greenlander denies anything with contempt or horror he turns up his nose,
and gives a slight sound through it.[9] Mr. Scott has sent me a graphic description of the face of a young
Hindoo at the sight of castoroil, which he was compelled occasionally to take. Mr. Scott has also seen the
same expression on the faces of highcaste natives who have approached close to some defiling object. Mr.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 100
Page No 103
Bridges says that the Fuegians "express contempt by shooting out the lips and hissing through them, and by
turning up the nose." The tendency either to snort through the nose, or to make a noise expressed by _ugh_ or
_ach_, is noticed by several of my correspondents.
[9] As quoted by Tylor, `Primitive Culture,' 1871, vol. i. p. 169.
Spitting seems an almost universal sign of contempt or disgust; and spitting obviously represents the rejection
of anything offensive from the mouth. Shakspeare makes the Duke of Norfolk say, "I spit at him call him a
slanderous coward and a villain." So, again, Falstaff says, "Tell thee what, Hal,if I tell thee a lie, spit in my
face." Leichhardt remarks that the Australians "interrupted their speeches by spitting, and uttering a noise like
pooh! pooh! apparently expressive of their disgust." And Captain Burton speaks of certain negroes "spitting
with disgust upon the ground." Captain Speedy informs me that this is likewise the case with the Abyssinians.
Mr. Geach says that with the Malays of Malacca the expression of disgust "answers to spitting from the
mouth;" and with the Fuegians, according to Mr. Bridges "to spit at one is the highest mark of contempt."
I never saw disgust more plainly expressed than on the face of one of my infants at the age of five months,
when, for the first time, some cold water, and again a month afterwards, when a piece of ripe cherry was put
into his mouth. This was shown by the lips and whole mouth assuming a shape which allowed the contents to
run or fall quickly out; the tongue being likewise protruded. These movements were accompanied by a little
shudder. It was all the more comical, as I doubt whether the child felt real disgust the eyes and forehead
expressing much surprise and consideration. The protrusion of the tongue in letting a nasty object fall out of
the mouth, may explain how it is that lolling out the tongue universally serves as a sign of contempt and
hatred.[11]
[10] Both these quotations are given by Mr. H. Wedgwood, `On the Origin of Language,' 1866, p. 75.
We have now seen that scorn, disdain, contempt, and disgust are expressed in many different ways, by
movements of the features, and by various gestures; and that these are the same throughout the world. They
all consist of actions representing the rejection or exclusion of some real object which we dislike or abhor,
but which does not excite in us certain other strong emotions, such as rage or terror; and through the force of
habit and association similar actions are performed, whenever any analogous sensation arises in our minds.
_Jealousy, Envy, Avarice, Revenge, Suspicion, Deceit, Slyness, Guilt, Vanity, Conceit, Ambition, Pride,
Humility, is doubtful whether the greater number of the above complex states of mind are revealed by any
fixed expression, sufficiently distinct to be described or delineated. When Shakspeare speaks of Envy as
_leanfaced_, or _black_, or _pale_, and Jealousy as "_the greeneyed monster_;" and when Spenser
describes Suspicion as "_foul, illfavoured, and grim_," they must have felt this difficulty. Nevertheless, the
above feelingsat least many of them can be detected by the eye; for instance, conceit; but we are often
guided in a much greater degree than we suppose by our previous knowledge of the persons or circumstances.
My correspondents almost unanimously answer in the affirmative to my query, whether the expression of
guilt and deceit can be recognized amongst the various races of man; and I have confidence in their answers,
as they generally deny that jealousy can thus be recognized. In the cases in which details are given, the eyes
are almost always referred to. The guilty man is said to avoid looking at his accuser, or to give him stolen
looks. The eyes are said "to be turned askant," or "to waver from side to side," or "the eyelids to be lowered
and partly closed." This latter remark is made by Mr. Hagenauer with respect to the Australians, and by
Gaika with respect to the Kafirs. The restless movements of the eyes apparently follow, as will be explained
when we treat of blushing, from the guilty man not enduring to meet the gaze of his accuser. I may add, that I
have observed a guilty expression, without a shade of fear, in some of my own children at a very early age. In
one instance the expression was unmistakably clear in a child two years and seven months old, and led to the
detection of his little crime. It was shown, as I record in my notes made at the time, by an unnatural
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 101
Page No 104
brightness in the eyes, and by an odd, affected manner, impossible to describe.
[11] This is stated to be the case by Mr. Tylor (Early Hist. of Mankind, 2nd edit. 1870, p. 52); and he adds, "it
is not clear why this should be so."
Slyness is also, I believe, exhibited chiefly by movements about the eyes; for these are less under the control
of the will, owing to the force of longcontinued habit, than are the movements of the body. Mr. Herbert
Spencer remarks,[12] "When there is a desire to see something on one side of the visual field without being
supposed to see it, the tendency is to check the conspicuous movement of the head, and to make the required
adjustment entirely with the eyes; which are, therefore, drawn very much to one side. Hence, when the eyes
are turned to one side, while the face is not turned to the same side, we get the natural language of what is
called slyness."
[12] `Principles of Psychology,' 2nd edit. 1872, p. 552.
Of all the abovenamed complex emotions, Pride, perhaps, is the most plainly expressed. A proud man
exhibits his sense of superiority over others by holding his head and body erect. He is haughty (_haut_), or
high, and makes himself appear as large as possible; so that metaphorically he is said to be swollen or puffed
up with pride. A peacock or a turkeycock strutting about with puffedup feathers, is sometimes said to be an
emblem of pride.[13] The arrogant man looks down on others, and with lowered eyelids hardly condescends
to see them; or he may show his contempt by slight movements, such as those before described, about the
nostrils or lips. Hence the muscle which everts the lower lip has been called the _musculus superbus_. In
some photographs of patients affected by a monomania of pride, sent me by Dr. Crichton Browne, the head
and body were held erect, and the mouth firmly closed. This latter action, expressive of decision, follows, I
presume, from the proud man feeling perfect selfconfidence in himself. The whole expression of pride
stands in direct antithesis to that of humility; so that nothing need here be said of the latter state of mind.
_Helplessness, Impotence: Shrugging the shoulders_.When a man wishes to show that he cannot do
something, or prevent something being done, he often raises with a quick movement both shoulders. At the
same time, if the whole gesture is completed, he bends his elbows closely inwards, raises his open hands,
turning them outwards, with the fingers separated. The head is often thrown a little on one side; the eyebrows
are elevated, and this causes wrinkles across the forehead. The mouth is generally opened. I may mention, in
order to show how unconsciously the features are thus acted on, that though I had often intentionally
shrugged my shoulders to observe how my arms were placed, I was not at all aware that my eyebrows were
raised and mouth opened, until I looked at myself in a glass; and since then I have noticed the same
movements in the faces of others. In the accompanying Plate VI., figs. 3 and 4, Mr. Rejlander has
successfully acted the gesture of shrugging the shoulders.
[12] Gratiolet (De la Phys. p. 351) makes this remark, and has some good observations on the expression of
pride. See Sir C. Bell (`Anatomy of Expression,' p. 111) on the action of the _musculus superbus_.
Englishmen are much less demonstrative than the men of most other European nations, and they shrug their
shoulders far less frequently and energetically than Frenchmen or Italians do. The gesture varies in all
degrees from the complex movement, just described, to only a momentary and scarcely perceptible raising of
both shoulders; or, as I have noticed in a lady sitting in an armchair, to the mere turning slightly outwards of
the open hands with separated fingers. I have never seen very young English children shrug their shoulders,
but the following case was observed with care by a medical professor and excellent observer, and has been
communicated to me by him. The father of this gentleman was a Parisian, and his mother a Scotch lady. His
wife is of British extraction on both sides, and my informant does not believe that she ever shrugged her
shoulders in her life. His children have been reared in England, and the nursemaid is a thorough
Englishwoman, who has never been seen to shrug her shoulders. Now, his eldest daughter was observed to
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 102
Page No 105
shrug her shoulders at the age of between sixteen and eighteen months; her mother exclaiming at the time,
"Look at the little French girl shrugging her shoulders!" At first she often acted thus, sometimes throwing her
head a little backwards and on one side, but she did not, as far as was observed, move her elbows and hands
in the usual manner. The habit gradually wore away, and now, when she is a little over four years old, she is
never seen to act thus. The father is told that he sometimes shrugs his shoulders, especially when arguing
with any one; but it is extremely improbable that his daughter should have imitated him at so early an age;
for, as he remarks, she could not possibly have often seen this gesture in him. Moreover, if the habit had been
acquired through imitation, it is not probable that it would so soon have been spontaneously discontinued by
this child, and, as we shall immediately see, by a second child, though the father still lived with his family.
This little girl, it may be added, resembles her Parisian grandfather in countenance to an almost absurd
degree. She also presents another and very curious resemblance to him, namely, by practising a singular trick.
When she impatiently wants something, she holds out her little hand, and rapidly rubs the thumb against the
index and middle finger: now this same trick was frequently performed under the same circumstances by her
grandfather.
This gentleman's second daughter also shrugged her shoulders before the age of eighteen months, and
afterwards discontinued the habit. It is of course possible that she may have imitated her elder sister; but she
continued it after her sister had lost the habit. She at first resembled her Parisian grandfather in a less degree
than did her sister at the same age, but now in a greater degree. She likewise practises to the present time the
peculiar habit of rubbing together, when impatient, her thumb and two of her forefingers.
In this latter case we have a good instance, like those given in a former chapter, of the inheritance of a trick or
gesture; for no one, I presume, will attribute to mere coincidence so peculiar a habit as this, which was
common to the grandfather and his two grandchildren who had never seen him.
Considering all the circumstances with reference to these children shrugging their shoulders, it can hardly be
doubted that they have inherited the habit from their French progenitors, although they have only one quarter
French blood in their veins, and although their grandfather did not often shrug his shoulders. There is nothing
very unusual, though the fact is interesting, in these children having gained by inheritance a habit during early
youth, and then discontinuing it; for it is of frequent occurrence with many kinds of animals that certain
characters are retained for a period by the young, and are then lost.
As it appeared to me at one time improbable in a high degree that so complex a gesture as shrugging the
shoulders, together with the accompanying movements, should be innate, I was anxious to ascertain whether
the blind and deaf Laura Bridgman, who could not have learnt the habit by imitation, practised it. And I have
heard, through Dr. Innes, from a lady who has lately had charge of her, that she does shrug her shoulders, turn
in her elbows, and raise her eyebrows in the same manner as other people, and under the same circumstances.
I was also anxious to learn whether this gesture was practised by the various races of man, especially by those
who never have had much intercourse with Europeans. We shall see that they act in this manner; but it
appears that the gesture is sometimes confined to merely raising or shrugging the shoulders, without the other
movements.
Mr. Scott has frequently seen this gesture in the Bengalees and Dhangars (the latter constituting a distinct
race) who are employed in the Botanic Garden at Calcutta; when, for instance, they have declared that they
could not do some work, such as lifting a heavy weight. He ordered a Bengalee to climb a lofty tree; but the
man, with a shrug of his shoulders and a lateral shake of his head, said he could not. Mr. Scott knowing that
the man was lazy, thought he could, and insisted on his trying. His face now became pale, his arms dropped
to his sides, his mouth and eyes were widely opened, and again surveying the tree, he looked askant at Mr.
Scott, shrugged his shoulders, inverted his elbows, extended his open hands, and with a few quick lateral
shakes of the head declared his inability. Mr. H. Erskine has likewise seen the natives of India shrugging their
shoulders; but he has never seen the elbows turned so much inwards as with us; and whilst shrugging their
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 103
Page No 106
shoulders they sometimes lay their uncrossed hands on their breasts.
With the wild Malays of the interior of Malacca, and with the Bugis (true Malays, though speaking a
different, language), Mr. Geach has often seen this gesture. I presume that it is complete, as, in answer to my
query descriptive of the movements of the shoulders, arms, hands, and face, Mr. Geach remarks, "it is
performed in a beautiful style." I have lost an extract from a scientific voyage, in which shrugging the
shoulders by some natives (Micronesians) of the Caroline Archipelago in the Pacific Ocean, was well
described. Capt. Speedy informs me that the Abyssinians shrug their shoulders but enters into no details. Mrs.
Asa Gray saw an Arab dragoman in Alexandria acting exactly as described in my query, when an old
gentleman, on whom he attended, would not go in the proper direction which had been pointed out to him.
Mr. Washington Matthews says, in reference to the wild Indian tribes of the western parts of the United
States, "I have on a few occasions detected men using a slight apologetic shrug, but the rest of the
demonstration which you describe I have not witnessed." Fritz Muller informs me that he has seen the
negroes in Brazil shrugging their shoulders; but it is of course possible that they may have learnt to do so by
imitating the Portuguese. Mrs. Barber has never seen this gesture with the Kafirs of South Africa; and Gaika,
judging from his answer, did not even understand what was meant by my description. Mr. Swinhoe is also
doubtful about the Chinese; but he has seen them, under the circumstances which would make us shrug our
shoulders, press their right elbow against their side, raise their eyebrows, lift up their hand with the palm
directed towards the person addressed, and shake it from right to left. Lastly, with respect to the Australians,
four of my informants answer by a simple negative, and one by a simple affirmative. Mr. Bunnett, who has
had excellent opportunities for observation on the borders of the Colony of Victory, also answers by a "yes,"
adding that the gesture is performed "in a more subdued and less demonstrative manner than is the case with
civilized nations." This circumstance may account for its not having been noticed by four of my informants.
These statements, relating to Europeans, Hindoos, the hilltribes of India, Malays, Micronesians,
Abyssinians, Arabs, Negroes, Indians of North America, and apparently to the Australiansmany of these
natives having had scarcely any intercourse with Europeansare sufficient to show that shrugging the
shoulders, accompanied in some cases by the other proper movements, is a gesture natural to mankind.
This gesture implies an unintentional or unavoidable action on our own part, or one that we cannot perform;
or an action performed by another person which we cannot prevent. It accompanies such speeches as, "It was
not my fault;" "It is impossible for me to grant this favour;" "He must follow his own course, I cannot stop
him." Shrugging the shoulders likewise expresses patience, or the absence of any intention to resist. Hence
the muscles which raise the shoulders are sometimes called, as I have been informed by an artist, the patience
muscles." Shylock the Jew, says,
"Signor Antonio, many a time and oft
In the Rialto have you rated me
About my monies and usances;
Still have I borne it with a patient shrug."
_Merchant of Venice_, act 1. sc. 3.
Sir C. Bell has given[14] a lifelike figure of a man, who is shrinking back from some terrible danger, and is
on the point of screaming out in abject terror. He is represented with his shoulders lifted up almost to his ears;
and this at once declares that there is no thought of resistance.
As shrugging the shoulders generally implies "I cannot do this or that," so by a slight change, it sometimes
implies "I won't do it." The movement then expresses a dogged determination not to act. Olmsted
describes[15] an Indian in Texas as giving a great shrug to his shoulders, when he was informed that a party
of men were Germans and not Americans, thus expressing that he would have nothing to do with them. Sulky
and obstinate children may be seen with both their shoulders raised high up; but this movement is not
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 104
Page No 107
associated with the others which generally accompany a true shrug. An excellent observer[16] in describing a
young man who was determined not to yield to his father's desire, says, "He thrust his hands deep down into
his pockets, and set up his shoulders to his ears, which was a good warning that, come right or wrong, this
rock should fly from its firm base as soon as Jack would; and that any remonstrance on the subject was purely
futile." As soon as the son got his own way, he "put his shoulders into their natural position."
[14] `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 166.
[15] `Journey through Texas,' p. 352.
Resignation is sometimes shown by the open hands being placed, one over the other, on the lower part of the
body. I should not have thought this little gesture worth even a passing notice, had not Dr. W. Ogle remarked
to me that he had two or three times observed it in patients who were preparing for operations under
chloroform. They exhibited no great fear, but seemed to declare by this posture of their hands, that they had
made up their minds, and were resigned to the inevitable.
We may now inquire why men in all parts of the world when they feel, whether or not they wish to show
this feeling,that they cannot or will not do something, or will not resist something if done by another,
shrug their shoulders, at the same time often bending in their elbows, showing the palms of their hands with
extended fingers, often throwing their heads a little on one side, raising their eyebrows, and opening their
mouths. These states of the mind are either simply passive, or show a determination not to act. None of the
above movements are of the least service. The explanation lies, I cannot doubt, in the principle of
unconscious antithesis. This principle here seems to come into play as clearly as in the case of a dog, who,
when feeling savage, puts himself in the proper attitude for attacking and for making himself appear terrible
to his enemy; but as soon as he feels affectionate, throws his whole body into a directly opposite attitude,
though this is of no direct use to him.
[16] Mrs. Oliphant, `The Brownlows,' vol. ii. p. 206.
Let it be observed how an indignant man, who resents, and will not submit to some injury, holds his head
erect, squares his shoulders, and expands his chest. He often clenches his fists, and puts one or both arms in
the proper position for attack or defence, with the muscles of his limbs rigid. He frowns,that is, he
contracts and lowers his brows,and, being determined, closes his mouth. The actions and attitude of a
helpless man are, in every one of these respects, exactly the reverse. In Plate VI. we may imagine one of the
figures on the left side to have just said, "What do you mean by insulting me?" and one of the figures on the
right side to answer, "I really could not help it." The helpless man unconsciously contracts the muscles of his
forehead which are antagonistic to those that cause a frown, and thus raises his eyebrows; at the same time he
relaxes the muscles about the mouth, so that the lower jaw drops. The antithesis is complete in every detail,
not only in the movements of the features, but in the position of the limbs and in the attitude of the whole
body, as may be seen in the accompanying plate. As the helpless or apologetic man often wishes to show his
state of mind, he then acts in a conspicuous or demonstrative manner.
In accordance with the fact that squaring the elbows and clenching the fists are gestures by no means
universal with the men of all races, when they feel indignant and are prepared to attack their enemy, so it
appears that a helpless or apologetic frame of mind is expressed in many parts of the world by merely
shrugging the shoulders, without turning inwards the elbows and opening the hands. The man or child who is
obstinate, or one who is resigned to some great misfortune, has in neither case any idea of resistance by active
means; and he expresses this state of mind, by simply keeping his shoulders raised; or he may possibly fold
his arms across his breast.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 105
Page No 108
_Signs of affirmation or approval, and of negation or disapproval: nodding and shaking the head_.I was
curious to ascertain how far the common signs used by us in affirmation and negation were general
throughout the world. These signs are indeed to a certain extent expressive of our feelings, as we give a
vertical nod of approval with a smile to our children, when we approve of their conduct; and shake our heads
laterally with a frown, when we disapprove. With infants, the first act of denial consists in refusing food; and
I repeatedly noticed with my own infants, that they did so by withdrawing their heads laterally from the
breast, or from anything offered them in a spoon. In accepting food and taking it into their mouths, they
incline their heads forwards. Since making these observations I have been informed that the same idea had
occurred to Charma.[17] It deserves notice that in accepting or taking food, there is only a single movement
forward, and a single nod implies an affirmation. On the other hand, in refusing food, especially if it be
pressed on them, children frequently move their heads several times from side to side, as we do in shaking
our heads in negation. Moreover, in the case of refusal, the head is not rarely thrown backwards, or the mouth
is closed, so that these movements might likewise come to serve as signs of negation. Mr. Wedgwood
remarks on this subject,[18] that "when the voice is exerted with closed teeth or lips, it produces the sound of
the letter _n_ or _m_. Hence we may account for the use of the particle _ne_ to signify negation, and possibly
also of the Greek mh in the same sense."
[17] `Essai sur le Langage,' 2nd edit. 1846. I am much indebted to Miss Wedgwood for having given me this
information, with an extract from the work.
That these signs are innate or instinctive, at least with AngloSaxons, is rendered highly probable by the
blind and deaf Laura Bridgman "constantly accompanying her _yes_ with the common affirmative nod, and
her _no_ with our negative shake of the head." Had not Mr. Lieber stated to the contrary,[19] I should have
imagined that these gestures might have been acquired or learnt by her, considering her wonderful sense of
touch and appreciation of the movements of others. With microcephalous idiots, who are so degraded that
they never learn to speak, one of them is described by Vogt,[20] as answering, when asked whether he
wished for more food or drink, by inclining or shaking his head. Schmalz, in his remarkable dissertation on
the education of the deaf and dumb, as well as of children raised only one degree above idiotcy, assumes that
they can always both make and understand the common signs of affirmation and negation."
Nevertheless if we look to the various races of man, these signs are not so universally employed as I should
have expected; yet they seem too general to be ranked as altogether conventional or artificial. My informants
assert that both signs are used by the Malays, by the natives of Ceylon, the Chinese, the negroes of the
Guinea coast, and, according to Gaika, by the Kafirs of South Africa, though with these latter people Mrs.
Barber has never seen a lateral shake used as a negative. With respect to the Australians, seven observers
agree that a nod is given in affirmation; five agree about a lateral shake in negation, accompanied or not by
some word; but Mr. Dyson Lacy has never seen this latter sign in Queensland, and Mr. Bulmer says that in
Gipps' Land a negative is expressed by throwing the head a little backwards and putting out the tongue. At
the northern extremity of the continent, near Torres Straits, the natives when uttering a negative "don't shake
the head with it, but holding up the right hand, shake it by turning it half round and back again two or three
times."[22] The throwing back of the head with a cluck of the tongue is said to be used as a negative by the
modern Greeks and Turks, the latter people expressing _yes_ by a movement like that made by us when we
shake our heads.[23] The Abyssinians, as I am informed by Captain Speedy, express a negative by jerking the
head to the right shoulder, together with a slight cluck, the mouth being closed; an affirmation is expressed by
the head being thrown backwards and the eyebrows raised for an instant. The Tagals of Luzon, in the
Philippine Archipelago, as I hear from Dr. Adolf Meyer, when they say "yes," also throw the head
backwards. According to the Rajah Brooke, the Dyaks of Borneo express an affirmation by raising the
eyebrows, and a negation by slightly contracting them, together with a peculiar look from the eyes. With the
Arabs on the Nile, Professor and Mrs. Asa Gray concluded that nodding in affirmation was rare, whilst
shaking the head in negation was never used, and was not even understood by them. With the Esquimaux[24]
a nod means _yes_ and a wink _no_. The New Zealanders "elevate the head and chin in place of nodding
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 106
Page No 109
acquiescence."[25]
[18] `On the Origin of Language,' 1866, p. 91.
[19] `On the Vocal Sounds of L. Bridgman;' Smithsonian Contributions, 1851, vol. ii. p. 11.
[20] `Memoire sur les Microcephales,' 1867, p. 27.
[21] Quoted by Tylor, `Early History of Mankind,' 2nd edit. 1870, p. 38.
[22] Mr. J. B. Jukes, `Letters and Extracts,' 1871, p. 248.
[23] F. Lieber, `On the Vocal Sounds,' p. 11. Tylor, ibid. p. 53.
With the Hindoos Mr. H. Erskine concludes from inquiries made from experienced Europeans, and from
native gentlemen, that the signs of affirmation and negation varya nod and a lateral shake being sometimes
used as we do; but a negative is more commonly expressed by the head being thrown suddenly backwards
and a little to one side, with a cluck of the tongue. What the meaning may be of this cluck of the tongue,
which has been observed with various people, I cannot imagine. A native gentleman stated that affirmation is
frequently shown by the head being thrown to the left. I asked Mr. Scott to attend particularly to this point,
and, after repeated observations, he believes that a vertical nod is not commonly used by the natives in
affirmation, but that the head is first thrown backwards either to the left or right, and then jerked obliquely
forwards only once. This movement would perhaps have been described by a less careful observer as a lateral
shake. He also states that in negation the head is usually held nearly upright, and shaken several times.
Mr. Bridges informs me that the Fuegians nod their heads vertically in affirmation, and shake them laterally
in denial. With the wild Indians of North America, according to Mr. Washington Matthews, nodding and
shaking the head have been learnt from Europeans, and are not naturally employed. They express affirmation
by describing with the hand (all the fingers except the index being flexed) a curve downwards and outwards
from the body, whilst negation is expressed by moving the open hand outwards, with the palm facing
inwards." Other observers state that the sign of affirmation with these Indians is the forefinger being raised,
and then lowered and pointed to the ground, or the hand is waved straight forward from the face; and that the
sign of negation is the finger or whole hand shaken from side to side.[26] This latter movement probably
represents in all cases the lateral shaking of the head. The Italians are said in like manner to move the lifted
finger from right to left in negation, as indeed we English sometimes do.
[24] Dr. King, Edinburgh Phil. Journal, 1845, p. 313.
[25] Tylor, `Early History of Mankind,' 2nd edit. 1870, p. 53.
On the whole we find considerable diversity in the signs of affirmation and negation in the different races of
man. With respect to negation, if we admit that the shaking of the finger or hand from side to side is symbolic
of the lateral movement of the head; and if we admit that the sudden backward movement of the head
represents one of the actions often practised by young children in refusing food, then there is much
uniformity throughout the world in the signs of negation, and we can see how they originated. The most
marked exceptions are presented by the Arabs, Esquimaux, some Australian tribes, and Dyaks. With the latter
a frown is the sign of negation, and with us frowning often accompanies a lateral shake of the head.
With respect to nodding in affirmation, the exceptions are rather more numerous, namely with some of the
Hindoos, with the Turks, Abyssinians, Dyaks, Tagals, and New Zealanders. The eyebrows are sometimes
raised in affirmation, and as a person in bending his head forwards and downwards naturally looks up to the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XI. DISDAINCONTEMPTDISGUSTGUILTPRIDE, ETC.HELPLESSNESSPATIENCE AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION. 107
Page No 110
person whom he addresses, he will be apt to raise his eyebrows, and this sign may thus have arisen as an
abbreviation. So again with the New Zealanders, the lifting up the chin and head in affirmation may perhaps
represent in an abbreviated form the upward movement of the head after it has been nodded forwards and
downwards.
[26] Lubbock, `The Origin of Civilization,' 1870, p. 277. Tylor, ibid. p. 38. Lieber (ibid. p. 11) remarks on the
negative of the Italians.
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR.
Surprise, astonishmentElevation of the eyebrowsOpening the mouth Protrusion of the lipsGestures
accompanying surprise AdmirationFearTerrorErection of the hairContraction of the platysma
muscleDilatation of the pupilsHorrorConclusion.
ATTENTION, if sudden and close, graduates into surprise; and this into astonishment; and this into stupefied
amazement. The latter frame of mind is closely akin to terror. Attention is shown by the eyebrows being
slightly raised; and as this state increases into surprise, they are raised to a much greater extent, with the eyes
and mouth widely open. The raising of the eyebrows is necessary in order that the eyes should be opened
quickly and widely; and this movement produces transverse wrinkles across the forehead. The degree to
which the eyes and mouth are opened corresponds with the degree of surprise felt; but these movements must
be coordinated; for a widely opened mouth with eyebrows only slightly raised results in a meaningless
grimace, as Dr. Duchenne has shown in one of his photographs.[1] On the other hand, a person may often be
seen to pretend surprise by merely raising his eyebrows.
Dr. Duchenne has given a photograph of an old man with his eyebrows well elevated and arched by the
galvanization of the frontal muscle; and with his mouth voluntarily opened. This figure expresses surprise
with much truth. I showed it to twentyfour persons without a word of explanation, and one alone did not at
all understand what was intended. A second person answered terror, which is not far wrong; some of the
others, however, added to the words surprise or astonishment, the epithets horrified, woful, painful, or
disgusted.
[1] `Mecanisme de la Physionomie,' Album, 1862, p. 42.
The eyes and mouth being widely open is an expression universally recognized as one of surprise or
astonishment. Thus Shakespeare says, "I saw a smith stand with open mouth swallowing a tailor's news."
(`King John,' act iv. scene ii.) And again, "They seemed almost, with staring on one another, to tear the cases
of their eyes; there was speech in the dumbness, language in their very gesture; they looked as they had heard
of a world destroyed." (`Winter's Tale,' act v. scene ii.)
My informants answer with remarkable uniformity to the same effect, with respect to the various races of
man; the above movements of the features being often accompanied by certain gestures and sounds, presently
to be described. Twelve observers in different parts of Australia agree on this head. Mr. Winwood Reade has
observed this expression with the negroes on the Guinea coast. The chief Gaika and others answer _yes_ to
my query with respect to the Kafirs of South Africa; and so do others emphatically with reference to the
Abyssinians, Ceylonese, Chinese, Fuegians, various tribes of North America, and New Zealanders. With the
latter, Mr. Stack states that the expression is more plainly shown by certain individuals than by others, though
all endeavour as much as possible to conceal their feelings. The Dyaks of Borneo are said by the Rajah
Brooke to open their eyes widely, when astonished, often swinging their heads to and fro, and beating their
breasts. Mr. Scott informs me that the workmen in the Botanic Gardens at Calcutta are strictly ordered not to
smoke; but they often disobey this order, and when suddenly surprised in the act, they first open their eyes
and mouths widely. They then often slightly shrug their shoulders, as they perceive that discovery is
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 108
Page No 111
inevitable, or frown and stamp on the ground from vexation. Soon they recover from their surprise, and abject
fear is exhibited by the relaxation of all their muscles; their heads seem to sink between their shoulders; their
fallen eyes wander to and fro; and they supplicate forgiveness.
The wellknown Australian explorer, Mr. Stuart, has given[2] a striking account of stupefied amazement
together with terror in a native who had never before seen a man on horseback. Mr. Stuart approached unseen
and called to him from a little distance. "He turned round and saw me. What he imagined I was I do not
know; but a finer picture of fear and astonishment I never saw. He stood incapable of moving a limb, riveted
to the spot, mouth open and eyes staring. . . . He remained motionless until our black got within a few yards
of him, when suddenly throwing down his waddies, he jumped into a mulga bush as high as he could get." He
could not speak, and answered not a word to the inquiries made by the black, but, trembling from head to
foot, "waved with his hand for us to be off."
That the eyebrows are raised by an innate or instinctive impulse may be inferred from the fact that Laura
Bridgman invariably acts thus when astonished, as I have been assured by the lady who has lately had charge
of her. As surprise is excited by something unexpected or unknown, we naturally desire, when startled, to
perceive the cause as quickly as possible; and we consequently open our eyes fully, so that the field of vision
may be increased, and the eyeballs moved easily in any direction. But this hardly accounts for the eyebrows
being so greatly raised as is the case, and for the wild staring of the open eyes. The explanation lies, I believe,
in the impossibility of opening the eyes with great rapidity by merely raising the upper lids. To effect this the
eyebrows must be lifted energetically. Any one who will try to open his eyes as quickly as possible before a
mirror will find that he acts thus; and the energetic lifting up of the eyebrows opens the eyes so widely that
they stare, the white being exposed all round the iris. Moreover, the elevation of the eyebrows is an advantage
in looking upwards; for as long as they are lowered they impede our vision in this direction. Sir C. Bell
gives[3] a curious little proof of the part which the eyebrows play in opening the eyelids. In a stupidly
drunken man all the muscles are relaxed, and the eyelids consequently droop, in the same manner as when we
are falling asleep. To counteract this tendency the drunkard raises his eyebrows; and this gives to him a
puzzled, foolish look, as is well represented in one of Hogarth's drawings. The habit of raising the eyebrows
having once been gained in order to see as quickly as possible all around us, the movement would follow
from the force of association whenever astonishment was felt from any cause, even from a sudden sound or
an idea.
[2] `The Polyglot News Letter,' Melbourne, Dec. 1858, p. 2.
With adult persons, when the eyebrows are raised, the whole forehead becomes much wrinkled in transverse
lines; but with children this occurs only to a slight degree. The wrinkles run in lines concentric with each
eyebrow, and are partially confluent in the middle. They are highly characteristic of the expression of surprise
or astonishment. Each eyebrow, when raised, becomes also, as Duchenne remarks,[4] more arched than it
was before.
[3] `The Anatomy of Expression,' p. 106.
The cause of the mouth being opened when astonishment is felt, is a much more complex affair; and several
causes apparently concur in leading to this movement. It has often been supposed[5] that the sense of hearing
is thus rendered more acute; but I have watched persons listening intently to a slight noise, the nature and
source of which they knew perfectly, and they did not open their mouths. Therefore I at one time imagined
that the open mouth might aid in distinguishing the direction whence a sound proceeded, by giving another
channel for its entrance into the ear through the eustachian tube, But Dr. W. Ogle[6] has been so kind as to
search the best recent authorities on the functions of the eustachian tube, and he informs me that it is almost
conclusively proved that it remains closed except during the act of deglutition; and that in persons in whom
the tube remains abnormally open, the sense of hearing, as far as external sounds are concerned, is by no
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 109
Page No 112
means improved; on the contrary, it is impaired by the respiratory sounds being rendered more distinct. If a
watch be placed within the mouth, but not allowed to touch the sides, the ticking is heard much less plainly
than when held outside. In persons in whom from disease or a cold the eustachian tube is permanently or
temporarily closed, the sense of hearing is injured; but this may be accounted for by mucus accumulating
within the tube, and the consequent exclusion of air. We may therefore infer that the mouth is not kept open
under the sense of astonishment for the sake of hearing sounds more distinctly; notwithstanding that most
deaf people keep their mouths open.
[4] Mecanisme de la Physionomie,' Album, p. 6.
[5] See, for instance, Dr. Piderit (`Mimik und Physiognomik,' s. 88), who has a good discussion on the
expression of surprise.
[6] Dr. Murie has also given me information leading to the same conclusion, derived in part from
comparative anatomy.
Every sudden emotion, including astonishment, quickens the action of the heart, and with it the respiration.
Now we can breathe, as Gratiolet remarks[7] and as appears to me to be the case, much more quietly through
the open mouth than through the nostrils. Therefore, when we wish to listen intently to any sound, we either
stop breathing, or breathe as quietly as possible, by opening our mouths, at the same time keeping our bodies
motionless. One of my sons was awakened in the night by a noise under circumstances which naturally led to
great care, and after a few minutes he perceived that his mouth was widely open. He then became conscious
that he had opened it for the sake of breathing as quietly as possible. This view receives support from the
reversed case which occurs with dogs. A dog when panting after exercise, or on a hot day, breathes loudly;
but if his attention be suddenly aroused, he instantly pricks his ears to listen, shuts his mouth, and breathes
quietly, as he is enabled to do, through his nostrils.
When the attention is concentrated for a length of time with fixed earnestness on any object or subject, all the
organs of the body are forgotten and neglected;[8] and as the nervous energy of each individual is limited in
amount, little is transmitted to any part of the system, excepting that which is at the time brought into
energetic action. Therefore many of the muscles tend to become relaxed, and the jaw drops from its own
weight. This will account for the dropping of the jaw and open mouth of a man stupefied with amazement,
and perhaps when less strongly affected. I have noticed this appearance, as I find recorded in my notes, in
very young children when they were only moderately surprised.
[7] `De la Physionomie,' 1865, p. 234.
[8] See, on this subject, Gratiolet, ibid. p. 254.
There is still another and highly effective cause, leading to the mouth being opened, when we are astonished,
and more especially when we are suddenly startled. We can draw a full and deep inspiration much more
easily through the widely open mouth than through the nostrils. Now when we start at any sudden sound or
sight, almost all the muscles of the body are involuntarily and momentarily thrown into strong action, for the
sake of guarding ourselves against or jumping away from the danger, which we habitually associate with
anything unexpected. But we always unconsciously prepare ourselves for any great exertion, as formerly
explained, by first taking a deep and full inspiration, and we consequently open our mouths. If no exertion
follows, and we still remain astonished, we cease for a time to breathe, or breathe as quietly as possible, in
order that every sound may be distinctly heard. Or again, if our attention continues long and earnestly
absorbed, all our muscles become relaxed, and the jaw, which was at first suddenly opened, remains dropped.
Thus several causes concur towards this same movement, whenever surprise, astonishment, or amazement is
felt.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 110
Page No 113
Although when thus affected, our mouths are generally opened, yet the lips are often a little protruded. This
fact reminds us of the same movement, though in a much more strongly marked degree, in the chimpanzee
and orang when astonished. As a strong expiration naturally follows the deep inspiration which accompanies
the first sense of startled surprise, and as the lips are often protruded, the various sounds which are then
commonly uttered can apparently be accounted for. But sometimes a strong expiration alone is heard; thus
Laura Bridgman, when amazed, rounds and protrudes her lips, opens them, and breathes strongly.[9] One of
the commonest sounds is a deep _Oh_; and this would naturally follow, as explained by Helmholtz, from the
mouth being moderately opened and the lips protruded. On a quiet night some rockets were fired from the
`Beagle,' in a little creek at Tahiti, to amuse the natives; and as each rocket, was let off there was absolute
silence, but this was invariably followed by a deep groaning _Oh_, resounding all round the bay. Mr.
Washington Matthews says that the North American Indians express astonishment by a groan; and the
negroes on the West Coast of Africa, according to Mr. Winwood Reade, protrude their lips, and make a
sound like _heigh, heigh_. If the mouth is not much opened, whilst the lips are considerably protruded, a
blowing, hissing, or whistling noise is produced. Mr. R. Brough Smith informs me that an Australian from
the interior was taken to the theatre to see an acrobat rapidly turning head over heels: "he was greatly
astonished, and protruded his lips, making a noise with his mouth as if blowing out a match." According to
Mr. Bulmer the Australians, when surprised, utter the exclamation _korki_, "and to do this the mouth is
drawn out as if going to whistle." We Europeans often whistle as a sign of surprise; thus, in a recent
novel[10] it is said, "here the man expressed his astonishment and disapprobation by a prolonged whistle." A
Kafir girl, as Mr. J. Mansel Weale informs me, "on hearing of the high price of an article, raised her
eyebrows and whistled just as a European would." Mr. Wedgwood remarks that such sounds are written
down as _whew_, and they serve as interjections for surprise.
[9] Lieber, `On the Vocal Sounds of Laura Bridgman,' Smithsonian Contributions, 1851, vol. ii. p. 7.
[10] `Wenderholme,' vol. ii. p. 91.
According to three other observers, the Australians often evince astonishment by a clucking noise. Europeans
also sometimes express gentle surprise by a little clicking noise of nearly the same kind. We have seen that
when we are startled, the mouth is suddenly opened; and if the tongue happens to be then pressed closely
against the palate, its sudden withdrawal will produce a sound of this kind, which might thus come to express
surprise.
Turning to gestures of the body. A surprised person often raises his opened hands high above his head, or by
bending his arms only to the level of his face. The flat palms are directed towards the person who causes this
feeling, and the straightened fingers are separated. This gesture is represented by Mr. Rejlander in Plate VII.
fig. 1. In the `Last Supper,' by Leonardo da Vinci, two of the Apostles have their hands half uplifted, clearly
expressive of their astonishment. A trustworthy observer told me that he had lately met his wife under most
unexpected circumstances: "She started, opened her mouth and eyes very widely, and threw up both her arms
above her head." Several years ago I was surprised by seeing several of my young children earnestly doing
something together on the ground; but the distance was too great for me to ask what they were about.
Therefore I threw up my open hands with extended fingers above my head; and as soon as I had done this, I
became conscious of the action. I then waited, without saying a word, to see if my children had understood
this gesture; and as they came running to me they cried out, "We saw that you were astonished at us." I do not
know whether this gesture is common to the various races of man, as I neglected to make inquiries on this
head. That it is innate or natural may be inferred from the fact that Laura Bridgman, when amazed, "spreads
her arms and turns her hands with extended fingers upwards;"[11] nor is it likely, considering that the feeling
of surprise is generally a brief one, that she should have learnt this gesture through her keen sense of touch.
Huschke describes[12] a somewhat different yet allied gesture, which he says is exhibited by persons when
astonished. They hold themselves erect, with the features as before described, but with the straightened arms
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 111
Page No 114
extended backwardsthe stretched fingers being separated from each other. I have never myself seen this
gesture; but Huschke is probably correct; for a friend asked another man how he would express great
astonishment, and he at once threw himself into this attitude.
These gestures are, I believe, explicable on the principle of antithesis. We have seen that an indignant man
holds his head erect, squares his shoulders, turns out his elbows, often clenches his fist, frowns, and closes his
mouth; whilst the attitude of a helpless man is in every one of these details the reverse. Now, a man in an
ordinary frame of mind, doing nothing and thinking of nothing in particular, usually keeps his two arms
suspended laxly by his sides, with his hands somewhat flexed, and the fingers near together. Therefore, to
raise the arms suddenly, either the whole arms or the forearms, to open the palms flat, and to separate the
fingers,or, again, to straighten the arms, extending them backwards with separated fingers,are
movements in complete antithesis to those preserved under an indifferent frame of mind, and they are, in
consequence, unconsciously assumed by an astonished man. There is, also, often a desire to display surprise
in a conspicuous manner, and the above attitudes are well fitted for this purpose. It may be asked why should
surprise, and only a few other states of the mind, be exhibited by movements in antithesis to others. But this
principle will not be brought into play in the case of those emotions, such as terror, great joy, suffering, or
rage, which naturally lead to certain lines of action and produce certain effects on the body, for the whole
system is thus preoccupied; and these emotions are already thus expressed with the greatest plainness.
[11] Lieber, `On the Vocal Sounds,' ibid. p. 7.
[12] Huschke, `Mimices et Physiognomices,' 1821, p. 18. Gratiolet (De la Phys. p. 255) gives a figure of a
man in this attitude, which, however, seems to me expressive of fear combined with astonishment. Le Brun
also refers (Lavater, vol. ix. p. 299) to the hands of an astonished man being opened.
There is another little gesture, expressive of astonishment of which I can offer no explanation; namely, the
hand being placed over the mouth or on some part of the head. This has been observed with so many races of
man, that it must have some natural origin. A wild Australian was taken into a large room full of official
papers, which surprised him greatly, and he cried out, _cluck, cluck, cluck_, putting the back of his hand
towards his lips. Mrs. Barber says that the Kafirs and Fingoes express astonishment by a serious look and by
placing the right hand upon the mouth, Littering the word _mawo_, which means `wonderful.' The Bushmen
are said[13] to put their right hands to their necks, bending their heads backwards. Mr. Winwood Reade has
observed that the negroes on the West Coast of Africa, when surprised, clap their hands to their mouths,
saying at the same time, "My mouth cleaves to me," i. e. to my hands; and he has heard that this is their usual
gesture on such occasions. Captain Speedy informs me that the Abyssinians place their right hand to the
forehead, with the palm outside. Lastly, Mr. Washington Matthews states that the conventional sign of
astonishment with the wild tribes of the western parts of the United States "is made by placing the
halfclosed hand over the mouth; in doing this, the head is often bent forwards, and words or low groans are
sometimes uttered." Catlin[14] makes the same remark about the hand being pressed over the mouth by the
Mandans and other Indian tribes.
[13] Huschke, ibid. p. 18.
_Admiration_.Little need be said on this head. Admiration apparently consists of surprise associated with
some pleasure and a sense of approval. When vividly felt, the eyes are opened and the eyebrows raised; the
eyes become bright, instead of remaining blank, as under simple astonishment; and the mouth, instead of
gaping open, expands into a smile.
_Fear, Terror_.The word `fear' seems to be derived from what is sudden and dangerous;[15] and that of
terror from the trembling of the vocal organs and body. I use the word `terror' for extreme fear; but some
writers think it ought to be confined to cases in which the imagination is more particularly concerned. Fear is
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 112
Page No 115
often preceded by astonishment, and is so far akin to it, that both lead to the senses of sight and hearing being
instantly aroused. In both cases the eyes and mouth are widely opened, and the eyebrows raised. The
frightened man at first stands like a statue motionless and breathless, or crouches down as if instinctively to
escape observation.
[14] `North American Indians,' 3rd edit. 1842, vol. i. p. 105.
[15] H. Wedgwood, Dict. of English Etymology, vol. ii. 1862, p. 35. See, also, Gratiolet (`De la
Physionomie,' p. 135) on the sources of such words as `terror, horror, rigidus, frigidus,'
The heart beats quickly and violently, so that it palpitates or knocks against the ribs; but it is very doubtful
whether it then works more efficiently than usual, so as to send a greater supply of blood to all parts of the
body; for the skin instantly becomes pale, as during incipient faintness. This paleness of the surface, however,
is probably in large part, or exclusively, due to the vasomotor centre being affected in such a manner as to
cause the contraction of the small arteries of the skin. That the skin is much affected under the sense of great
fear, we see in the marvellous and inexplicable manner in which perspiration immediately exudes from it.
This exudation is all the more remarkable, as the surface is then cold, and hence the term a cold sweat;
whereas, the sudorific glands are properly excited into action when the surface is heated. The hairs also on
the skin stand erect; and the superficial muscles shiver. In connection with the disturbed action of the heart,
the breathing is hurried. The salivary glands act imperfectly; the mouth becomes dry,[16] and is often opened
and shut. I have also noticed that under slight fear there is a strong tendency to yawn. One of the bestmarked
symptoms is the trembling of all the muscles of the body; and this is often first seen in the lips. From this
cause, and from the dryness of the mouth, the voice becomes husky or indistinct, or may altogether fail.
"Obstupui, steteruntque comae, et vox faucibus haesit."
[16] Mr. Bain (`The Emotions and the Will,' 1865, p. 54) explains in the following manner the origin of the
custom "of subjecting criminals in India to the ordeal of the morsel of rice. The accused is made to take a
mouthful of rice, and after a little time to throw it out. If the morsel is quite dry, the party is believed to be
guilty, his own evil conscience operating to paralyse the salivating organs."
Of vague fear there is a wellknown and grand description in Job:"In thoughts from the visions of the
night, when deep sleep falleth on men, fear came upon me, and trembling, which made all my bones to shake.
Then a spirit passed before my face; the hair of my flesh stood up. It stood still, but I could not discern the
form thereof: an image was before my eyes, there was silence, and I heard a voice, saying, Shall mortal man
be more just than God? Shall a man be more pure than his Maker?" (Job iv. 13)
As fear increases into an agony of terror, we behold, as under all violent emotions, diversified results. The
heart beats wildly, or may fail to act and faintness ensue; there is a deathlike pallor; the breathing is
laboured; the wings of the nostrils are wildly dilated; "there is a gasping and convulsive motion of the lips, a
tremor on the hollow cheek, a gulping and catching of the throat;"[17] the uncovered and protruding eyeballs
are fixed on the object of terror; or they may roll restlessly from side to side, _huc illuc volvens oculos
totumque pererrat_.[18] The pupils are said to be enormously dilated. All the muscles of the body may
become rigid, or may be thrown into convulsive movements. The hands are alternately clenched and opened,
often with a twitching movement. The arms may be protruded, as if to avert some dreadful danger, or may be
thrown wildly over the head. The Rev. Mr. Hagenauer has seen this latter action in a terrified Australian. In
other cases there is a sudden and uncontrollable tendency to headlong flight; and so strong is this, that the
boldest soldiers may be seized with a sudden panic.
[17] Sir C. Bell, Transactions of Royal Phil. Soc. 1822, p. 308. `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 88 and pp.
164469.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 113
Page No 116
[18] See Moreau on the rolling of the eyes, in the edit. of 1820 of Lavater, tome iv. p. 263. Also, Gratiolet,
De la Phys. p. 17.
As fear rises to an extreme pitch, the dreadful scream of terror is heard. Great beads of sweat stand on the
skin. All the muscles of the body are relaxed. Utter prostration soon follows, and the mental powers fail. The
intestines are affected. The sphincter muscles cease to act, and no longer retain the contents of the body.
Dr. J. Crichton Browne has given me so striking an account of intense fear in an insane woman, aged
thirtyfive, that the description though painful ought not to be omitted. When a paroxysm seizes her, she
screams out, "This is hell!" "There is a black woman!" "I can't get out!"and other such exclamations. When
thus screaming, her movements are those of alternate tension and tremor. For one instant she clenches her
hands, holds her arms out before her in a stiff semiflexed position; then suddenly bends her body forwards,
sways rapidly to and fro, draws her fingers through her hair, clutches at her neck, and tries to tear off her
clothes. The sternocleidomastoid muscles (which serve to bend the head on the chest) stand out
prominently, as if swollen, and the skin in front of them is much wrinkled. Her hair, which is cut short at the
back of her head, and is smooth when she is calm, now stands on end; that in front being dishevelled by the
movements of her hands. The countenance expresses great mental agony. The skin is flushed over the face
and neck, down to the clavicles, and the veins of the forehead and neck stand out like thick cords. The lower
lip drops, and is somewhat everted. The mouth is kept half open, with the lower jaw projecting. The cheeks
are hollow and deeply furrowed in curved lines running from the wings of the nostrils to the corners of the
mouth. The nostrils themselves are raised and extended. The eyes are widely opened, and beneath them the
skin appears swollen; the pupils are large. The forehead is wrinkled transversely in many folds, and at the
inner extremities of the eyebrows it is strongly furrowed in diverging lines, produced by the powerful and
persistent contraction of the corrugators.
Mr. Bell has also described[19] an agony of terror and of despair, which he witnessed in a murderer, whilst
carried to the place of execution in Turin. "On each side of the car the officiating priests were seated; and in
the centre sat the criminal himself. It was impossible to witness the condition of this unhappy wretch without
terror; and yet, as if impelled by some strange infatuation, it was equally impossible not to gaze upon an
object so wild, so full of horror. He seemed about thirtyfive years of age; of large and muscular form; his
countenance marked by strong and savage features; half naked, pale as death, agonized with terror, every
limb strained in anguish, his hands clenched convulsively, the sweat breaking out on his bent and contracted
brow, he kissed incessantly the figure of our Saviour, painted on the flag which was suspended before him;
but with an agony of wildness and despair, of which nothing ever exhibited on the stage can give the slightest
conception."
I will add only one other case, illustrative of a man utterly prostrated by terror. An atrocious murderer of two
persons was brought into a hospital, under the mistaken impression that he had poisoned himself; and Dr. W.
Ogle carefully watched him the next morning, while he was being handcuffed and taken away by the police.
His pallor was extreme, and his prostration so great that he was hardly able to dress himself. His skin
perspired; and his eyelids and head drooped so much that it was impossible to catch even a glimpse of his
eyes. His lower jaw hung down. There was no contraction of any facial muscle, and Dr. Ogle is almost
certain that the hair did not stand on end, for he observed it narrowly, as it had been dyed for the sake of
concealment.
[19] `Observations on Italy,' 1825, p. 48, as quoted in 'The Anatomy of Expression,' p. 168.
With respect to fear, as exhibited by the various races of man, my informants agree that the signs are the
same as with Europeans. They are displayed in an exaggerated degree with the Hindoos and natives of
Ceylon. Mr. Geach has seen Malays when terrified turn pale and shake; and Mr. Brough Smyth states that a
native Australian "being on one occasion much frightened, showed a complexion as nearly approaching to
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 114
Page No 117
what we call paleness, as can well be conceived in the case of a very black man." Mr. Dyson Lacy has seen
extreme fear shown in an Australian, by a nervous twitching of the hands, feet, and lips; and by the
perspiration standing on the skin. Many savages do not repress the signs of fear so much as Europeans; and
they often tremble greatly. With the Kafir, Gaika says, in his rather quaint English, the shaking "of the body
is much experienced, and the eyes are widely open." With savages, the sphincter muscles are often relaxed,
just as may be observed in much frightened dogs, and as I have seen with monkeys when terrified by being
caught.
_The erection of the hair_.Some of the signs of fear deserve a little further consideration. Poets continually
speak of the hair standing on end; Brutus says to the ghost of Caesar, "that mak'st my blood cold, and my hair
to stare." And Cardinal Beaufort, after the murder of Gloucester exclaims, "Comb down his hair; look, look,
it stands upright." As I did not feel sure whether writers of fiction might not have applied to man what they
had often observed in animals, I begged for information from Dr. Crichton Browne with respect to the insane.
He states in answer that he has repeatedly seen their hair erected under the influence of sudden and extreme
terror. For instance, it is occasionally necessary to inject morphia, under the skin of an insane woman, who
dreads the operation extremely, though it causes very little pain; for she believes that poison is being
introduced into her system, and that her bones will be softened, and her flesh turned into dust. She becomes
deadly pale; her limbs are stiffened by a sort of tetanic spasm, and her hair is partially erected on the front of
the head.
Dr. Browne further remarks that the bristling of the hair which is so common in the insane, is not always
associated with terror. It is perhaps most frequently seen in chronic maniacs, who rave incoherently and have
destructive impulses; but it is during their paroxysms of violence that the bristling is most observable. The
fact of the hair becoming erect under the influence both of rage and fear agrees perfectly with what we have
seen in the lower animals. Dr. Browne adduces several cases in evidence. Thus with a man now in the
Asylum, before the recurrence of each maniacal paroxysm, "the hair rises up from his forehead like the mane
of a Shetland pony." He has sent me photographs of two women, taken in the intervals between their
paroxysms, and he adds with respect to one of these women, "that the state of her hair is a sure and
convenient criterion of her mental condition." I have had one of these photographs copied, and the engraving
gives, if viewed from a little distance, a faithful representation of the original, with the exception that the hair
appears rather too coarse and too much curled. The extraordinary condition of the hair in the insane is due,
not only to its erection, but to its dryness and harshness, consequent on the subcutaneous glands failing to act.
Dr. Bucknill has said[20] that a lunatic "is a lunatic to his finger's ends;" he might have added, and often to
the extremity of each particular hair.
Dr. Browne mentions as an empirical confirmation of the relation which exists in the insane between the state
of their hair and minds, that the wife of a medical man, who has charge of a lady suffering from acute
melancholia, with a strong fear of death, for herself, her husband and children, reported verbally to him the
day before receiving my letter as follows, "I think Mrs. will soon improve, for her hair is getting
smooth; and I always notice that our patients get better whenever their hair ceases to be rough and
unmanageable."
Dr. Browne attributes the persistently rough condition of the hair in many insane patients, in part to their
minds being always somewhat disturbed, and in part to the effects of habit,that is, to the hair being
frequently and strongly erected during their many recurrent paroxysms. In patients in whom the bristling of
the hair is extreme, the disease is generally permanent and mortal; but in others, in whom the bristling is
moderate, as soon as they recover their health of mind the hair recovers its smoothness.
[20] Quoted by Dr. Maudsley, `Body and Mind,' 1870, p. 41.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 115
Page No 118
In a previous chapter we have seen that with animals the hairs are erected by the contraction of minute,
unstriped, and involuntary muscles, which run to each separate follicle. In addition to this action, Mr. J.
Wood has clearly ascertained by experiment, as he informs me, that with man the hairs on the front of the
head which slope forwards, and those on the back which slope backwards, are raised in opposite directions by
the contraction of the occipitofrontalis or scalp muscle. So that this muscle seems to aid in the erection of
the hairs on the head of man. in the same manner as the homologous _panniculus carnosus_ aids, or takes the
greater part, in the erection of the spines on the backs of some of the lower animals.
_Contraction of the platysma myoides muscle_.This muscle is spread over the sides of the neck, extending
downwards to a little beneath the collarbones, and upwards to the lower part of the cheeks. A portion, called
the risorius, is represented in the woodcut (M) fig. 2. The contraction of this muscle draws the corners of the
mouth and the lower parts of the checks downwards and backwards. It produces at the same time divergent,
longitudinal, prominent ridges on the sides of the neck in the young; and, in old thin persons, fine transverse
wrinkles. This muscle is sometimes said not to be under the control of the will; but almost every one, if told
to draw the corners of his mouth backwards and downwards with great force, brings it into action. I have,
however, heard of a man who can voluntarily act on it only on one side of his neck.
Sir C. Bell[21] and others have stated that this muscle is strongly contracted under the influence of fear; and
Duchenne insists so strongly on its importance in the expression of this emotion, that he calls it the _muscle
of fright_.[22] He admits, however, that its contraction is quite inexpressive unless associated with widely
open eyes and mouth. He has given a photograph (copied and reduced in the accompanying woodcut) of the
same old man as on former occasions, with his eyebrows strongly raised, his mouth opened, and the platysma
contracted, all by means of galvanism. The original photograph was shown to twentyfour persons, and they
were separately asked, without any explanation being given, what expression was intended: twenty instantly
answered, "intense fright" or "horror;" three said pain, and one extreme discomfort. Dr. Duchenne has given
another photograph of the same old man, with the platysma contracted, the eyes and mouth opened, and the
eyebrows rendered oblique, by means of galvanism. The expression thus induced is very striking (see Plate
VII. fig. 2); the obliquity of the eyebrows adding the appearance of great mental distress. The original was
shown to fifteen persons; twelve answered terror or horror, and three agony or great suffering. From these
cases, and from an examination of the other photographs given by Dr. Duchenne, together with his remarks
thereon, I think there can be little doubt that the contraction of the platysma does add greatly to the
expression of fear. Nevertheless this muscle ought hardly to be called that of fright, for its contraction is
certainly not a necessary concomitant of this state of mind.
[21] `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 168.
[22] Mecanisme de la Phys. Humaine, Album, Legende xi. A man may exhibit extreme terror in the plainest
manner by deathlike pallor, by drops of perspiration on his skin, and by utter prostration, with all the
muscles of his body, including the platysma, completely relaxed. Although Dr. Browne has often seen this
muscle quivering and contracting in the insane, he has not been able to connect its action with any emotional
condition in them, though he carefully attended to patients suffering from great fear. Mr. Nicol, on the other
hand, has observed three cases in which this muscle appeared to be more or less permanently contracted
under the influence of melancholia, associated with much dread; but in one of these cases, various other
muscles about the neck and head were subject to spasmodic contractions.
Dr. W. Ogle observed for me in one of the London hospitals about twenty patients, just before they were put
under the influence of chloroform for operations. They exhibited some trepidation, but no great terror. In only
four of the cases was the platysma visibly contracted; and it did not begin to contract until the patients began
to cry. The muscle seemed to contract at the moment of each deepdrawn inspiration; so that it is very
doubtful whether the contraction depended at all on the emotion of fear. In a fifth case, the patient, who was
not chloroformed, was much terrified; and his platysma was more forcibly and persistently contracted than in
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 116
Page No 119
the other cases. But even here there is room for doubt, for the muscle which appeared to be unusually
developed, was seen by Dr. Ogle to contract as the man moved his head from the pillow, after the operation
was over.
As I felt much perplexed why, in any case, a superficial muscle on the neck should be especially affected by
fear, I applied to my many obliging correspondents for information about the contraction of this muscle under
other circumstances. It would be superfluous to give all the answers which I have received. They show that
this muscle acts, often in a variable manner and degree, under many different conditions. It is violently
contracted in hydrophobia, and in a somewhat less degree in lockjaw; sometimes in a marked manner during
the insensibility from chloroform. Dr. W. Ogle observed two male patients, suffering from such difficulty in
breathing, that the trachea had to be opened, and in both the platysma was strongly contracted. One of these
men overheard the conversation of the surgeons surrounding him, and when he was able to speak, declared
that he had not been frightened. In some other cases of extreme difficulty of respiration, though not requiring
tracheotomy, observed by Drs. Ogle and Langstaff, the platysma was not contracted.
Mr. J. Wood, who has studied with such care the muscles of the human body, as shown by his various
publications, has often seen the platysma contracted in vomiting, nausea, and disgust; also in children and
adults under the influence of rage,for instance, in Irishwomen, quarrelling and brawling together with
angry gesticulations. This may possibly have been due to their high and angry tones; for I know a lady, an
excellent musician, who, in singing certain high notes, always contracts her platysma. So does a young man,
as I have observed, in sounding certain notes on the flute. Mr. J. Wood informs me that he has found the
platysma best developed in persons with thick necks and broad shoulders; and that in families inheriting these
peculiarities, its development is usually associated with much voluntary power over the homologous
occipitofrontalis muscle, by which the scalp can be moved.
None of the foregoing cases appear to throw any light on the contraction of the platysma from fear; but it is
different, I think, with the following cases. The gentleman before referred to, who can voluntarily act on this
muscle only on one side of his neck, is positive that it contracts on both sides whenever he is startled.
Evidence has already been given showing that this muscle sometimes contracts, perhaps for the sake of
opening the mouth widely, when the breathing is rendered difficult by disease, and during the deep
inspirations of cryingfits before an operation. Now, whenever a person starts at any sudden sight or sound,
he instantaneously draws a deep breath; and thus the contraction of the platysma may possibly have become
associated with the sense of fear. But there is, I believe, a more efficient relation. The first sensation of fear,
or the imagination of something dreadful, commonly excites a shudder. I have caught myself giving a little
involuntary shudder at a painful thought, and I distinctly perceived that my platysma contracted; so it does if
I simulate a shudder. I have asked others to act in this manner; and in some the muscle contracted, but not in
others. One of my sons, whilst getting out of bed, shuddered from the cold, and, as he happened to have his
hand on his neck, he plainly felt that this muscle strongly contracted. He then voluntarily shuddered, as he
had done on former occasions, but the platysma was not then affected. Mr. J. Wood has also several times
observed this muscle contracting in patients, when stripped for examination, and who were not frightened,
but shivered slightly from the cold. Unfortunately I have not been able to ascertain whether, when the whole
body shakes, as in the cold stage of an ague fit, the platysma contracts. But as it certainly often contracts
during a shudder; and as a shudder or shiver often accompanies the first sensation of fear, we have, I think, a
clue to its action in this latter case.[23] Its contraction, however, is not an invariable concomitant of fear; for
it probably never acts under the influence of extreme, prostrating terror.
[23] Ducheinne takes, in fact, this view (ibid. p. 45), as he attributes the contraction of the platysma to the
shivering of fear (_frisson de la peur_); but he elsewhere compares the action with that which causes the hair
of frightened quadrupeds to stand erect; and this can hardly be considered as quite correct.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 117
Page No 120
_Dilatation of the Pupils_.Gratiolet repeatedly insists[24] that the pupils are enormously dilated whenever
terror is felt. I have no reason to doubt the accuracy of this statement, but have failed to obtain confirmatory
evidence, excepting in the one instance before given of an insane woman suffering from great fear. When
writers of fiction speak of the eyes being widely dilated, I presume that they refer to the eyelids. Munro's
statement," that with parrots the iris is affected by the passions, independently of the amount of light, seems
to bear on this question; but Professor Donders informs me, that he has often seen movements in the pupils of
these birds which he thinks may be related to their power of accommodation to distance, in nearly the same
manner as our own pupils contract when our eyes converge for near vision. Gratiolet remarks that the dilated
pupils appear as if they were gazing into profound darkness. No doubt the fears of man have often been
excited in the dark; but hardly so often or so exclusively, as to account for a fixed and associated habit having
thus arisen. It seems more probable, assuming that Gratiolet's statement is correct, that the brain is directly
affected by the powerful emotion of fear and reacts on the pupils; but Professor Donders informs me that this
is an extremely complicated subject. I may add, as possibly throwing light on the subject, that Dr. Fyffe, of
Netley Hospital, has observed in two patients that the pupils were distinctly dilated during the cold stage of
an ague fit. Professor Donders has also often seen dilatation of the pupils in incipient faintness.
[24] `De la Physionomie,' pp. 51, 256, 346.
[25] As quoted in White's `Gradation in Man,' p. 57.
_Horror_.The state of mind expressed by this term implies terror, and is in some, cases almost
synonymous with it. Many a man must have felt, before the blessed discovery of chloroform, great horror at
the thought of an impending surgical operation. He who dreads, as well as hates a man, will feel, as Milton
uses the word, a horror of him. We feel horror if we see any one, for instance a child, exposed to some instant
and crushing danger. Almost every one would experience the same feeling in the highest degree in witnessing
a man being tortured or going to be tortured. In these cases there is no danger to ourselves; but from the
power of the imagination and of sympathy we put ourselves in the position of the sufferer, and feel something
akin to fear.
Sir C. Bell remarks,[26] that "horror is full of energy; the body is in the utmost tension, not unnerved by
fear." It is, therefore, probable that horror would generally be accompanied by the strong contraction of the
brows; but as fear is one of the elements, the eyes and mouth would be opened, and the eyebrows would be
raised, as far as the antagonistic action of the corrugators permitted this movement. Duchenne has given a
photograph[27] (fig. 21) of the same old man as before, with his eyes somewhat staring, the eyebrows
partially raised, and at the same time strongly contracted, the mouth opened, and the platysma in action, all
effected by the means of galvanism. He considers that the expression thus produced shows extreme terror
with horrible pain or torture. A tortured man, as long as his sufferings allowed him to feel any dread for the
future, would probably exhibit horror in an extreme degree. I have shown the original of this photograph to
twentythree persons of both sexes and various ages; and thirteen immediately answered horror, great pain,
torture, or agony; three answered extreme fright; so that sixteen answered nearly in accordance with
Duchenne's belief. Six, however, said anger, guided no doubt, by the strongly contracted brows, and
overlooking the peculiarly opened mouth. One said disgust. On the whole, the evidence indicates that we
have here a fairly good representation of horror and agony. The photograph before referred to (Pl. VII. fig. 2)
likewise exhibits horror; but in this the oblique eyebrows indicate great mental distress in place of energy.
[26] `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 169.
[27] `Mecanisme de la Physionomie,' Album, pl. 65, pp. 44, 45.
Horror is generally accompanied by various gestures, which differ in different individuals. Judging from
pictures, the whole body is often turned away or shrinks; or the arms are violently protruded as if to push
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XII. SURPRISEASTONISHMENTFEARHORROR. 118
Page No 121
away some dreadful object. The most frequent gesture, as far as can be inferred from the action of persons
who endeavour to express a vividlyimagined scene of horror, is the raising of both shoulders, with the bent
arms pressed closely against the sides or chest. These movements are nearly the same with those commonly
made when we feel very cold; and they are generally accompanied by a shudder, as well as by a deep
expiration or inspiration, according as the chest happens at the time to be expanded or contracted. The sounds
thus made are expressed by words like _uh_ or _ugh_.[28] It is not, however, obvious why, when we feel
cold or express a sense of horror, we press our bent arms against our bodies, raise our shoulders, and shudder.
[28] See remarks to this effect by Mr. Wedgwood, in the Introduction to his `Dictionary of English
Etymology,' 2nd edit. 1872, p. xxxvii. He shows by intermediate forms that the sounds here referred to have
probably given rise to many words, such as _ugly, huge_, _Conclusion_.I have now endeavoured to
describe the diversified expressions of fear, in its gradations from mere attention to a start of surprise, into
extreme terror and horror. Some of the signs may be accounted for through the principles of habit,
association, and inheritance,such as the wide opening of the mouth and eyes, with upraised eyebrows, so
as to see as quickly as possible all around us, and to hear distinctly whatever sound may reach our ears. For
we have thus habitually prepared ourselves to discover and encounter any danger. Some of the other signs of
fear may likewise be accounted for, at least in part, through these same principles. Men, during numberless
generations, have endeavoured to escape from their enemies or danger by headlong flight, or by violently
struggling with them; and such great exertions will have caused the heart to beat rapidly, the breathing to be
hurried, the chest to heave, and the nostrils to be dilated. As these exertions have often been prolonged to the
last extremity, the final result will have been utter prostration, pallor, perspiration, trembling of all the
muscles, or their complete relaxation. And now, whenever the emotion of fear is strongly felt, though it may
not lead to any exertion, the same results tend to reappear, through the force of inheritance and association.
Nevertheless, it is probable that many or most of the above symptoms of terror, such as the beating of the
heart, the trembling of the muscles, cold perspiration, are in large part directly due to the disturbed or
interrupted transmission of nerveforce from the cerebrospinal system to various parts of the body, owing to
the mind being so powerfully affected. We may confidently look to this cause, independently of habit and
association, in such cases as the modified secretions of the intestinal canal, and the failure of certain glands to
act. With respect to the involuntary bristling of the hair, we have good reason to believe that in the case of
animals this action, however it may have originated, serves, together with certain voluntary movements, to
make them appear terrible to their enemies; and as the same involuntary and voluntary actions are performed
by animals nearly related to man, we are led to believe that man has retained through inheritance a relic of
them, now become useless. It is certainly a remarkable fact, that the minute unstriped muscles, by which the
hairs thinly scattered over man's almost naked body are erected, should have been preserved to the present
day; and that they should still contract under the same emotions, namely, terror and rage, which cause the
hairs to stand on end in the lower members of the Order to which man belongs.
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY:
BLUSHING.
Nature of a blushInheritanceThe parts of the body most affected Blushing in the various races of
manAccompanying gestures Confusion of mindCauses of blushingSelfattention, the fundamental
elementShynessShame, from broken moral laws and conventional rulesModestyTheory of
blushingRecapitulation.
BLUSHING is the most peculiar and the most human of all expressions. Monkeys redden from passion, but it
would require an overwhelming amount of evidence to make us believe that any animal could blush. The
reddening of the face from a blush is due to the relaxation of the muscular coats of the small arteries, by
which the capillaries become filled with blood; and this depends on the proper vasomotor centre being
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 119
Page No 122
affected. No doubt if there be at the same time much mental agitation, the general circulation will be affected;
but it is not due to the action of the heart that the network of minute vessels covering the face becomes under
a sense of shame gorged with blood. We can cause laughing by tickling the skin, weeping or frowning by a
blow, trembling from the fear of pain, and so forth; but we cannot cause a blush, as Dr. Burgess remarks,[1]
by any physical means,that is by any action on the body. It is the mind which must be affected. Blushing is
not only involuntary; but the wish to restrain it, by leading to selfattention actually increases the tendency.
[1] `The Physiology or Mechanism of Blushing,' 1839, p. 156. I shall have occasion often to quote this work
in the present chapter.
The young blush much more freely than the old, but not during infancy,[2] which is remarkable, as we know
that infants at a very early age redden from passion. I have received authentic accounts of two little girls
blushing at the ages of between two and three years; and of another sensitive child, a year older, blushing,
when reproved for a fault. Many children, at a somewhat more advanced age blush in a strongly marked
manner. It appears that the mental powers of infants are not as yet sufficiently developed to allow of their
blushing. Hence, also, it is that idiots rarely blush. Dr. Crichton Browne observed for me those under his care,
but never saw a genuine blush, though he has seen their faces flash, apparently from joy, when food was
placed before them, and from anger. Nevertheless some, if not utterly degraded, are capable of blushing. A
microcephalous idiot, for instance, thirteen years old, whose eyes brightened a little when he was pleased or
amused, has been described by Dr. Behn,[3] as blushing and turning to one side, when undressed for medical
examination.
Women blush much more than men. It is rare to see an old man, but not nearly so rare to see an old woman
blushing. The blind do not escape. Laura Bridgman, born in this condition, as well as completely deaf,
blushes.[4] The Rev. R. H. Blair, Principal of the Worcester College, informs me that three children born
blind, out of seven or eight then in the Asylum, are great blushers. The blind are not at first conscious that
they are observed, and it is a most important part of their education, as Mr. Blair informs me, to impress this
knowledge on their minds; and the impression thus gained would greatly strengthen the tendency to blush, by
increasing the habit of selfattention.
[2] Dr. Burgess, ibid. p. 56. At p. 33 he also remarks on women blushing more freely than men, as stated
below.
[3] Quoted by Vogt, `Memoire sur les Microcephales,' 1867, p. 20. Dr. Burgess (ibid. p. 56) doubts whether
idiots ever blush.
The tendency to blush is inherited. Dr. Burgess gives the case[5] of a family consisting of a father, mother,
and ten children, all of whom, without exception, were prone to blush to a most painful degree. The children
were grown up; "and some of them were sent to travel in order to wear away this diseased sensibility, but
nothing was of the slightest avail." Even peculiarities in blushing seem to be inherited. Sir James Paget,
whilst examining the spine of a girl, was struck at her singular manner of blushing; a big splash of red
appeared first on one cheek, and then other splashes, variously scattered over the face and neck. He
subsequently asked the mother whether her daughter always blushed in this peculiar manner; and was
answered, "Yes, she takes after me." Sir J. Paget then perceived that by asking this question he had caused the
mother to blush; and she exhibited the same peculiarity as her daughter.
In most cases the face, ears and neck are the sole parts which redden; but many persons, whilst blushing
intensely, feel that their whole bodies grow hot and tingle; and this shows that the entire surface must be in
some manner affected. Blushes are said sometimes to commence on the forehead, but more commonly on the
cheeks, afterwards spreading to the ears and neck.[6] In two Albinos examined by Dr. Burgess, the blushes
commenced by a small circumscribed spot on the cheeks, over the parotidean plexus of nerves, and then
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 120
Page No 123
increased into a circle; between this blushing circle and the blush on the neck there was an evident line of
demarcation; although both arose simultaneously. The retina, which is naturally red in the Albino, invariably
increased at the same time in redness.[7] Every one must have noticed how easily after one blush fresh
blushes chase each other over the face. Blushing is preceded by a peculiar sensation in the skin. According to
Dr. Burgess the reddening of the skin is generally succeeded by a slight pallor, which shows that the capillary
vessels contract after dilating. In some rare cases paleness instead of redness is caused under conditions
which would naturally induce a blush. For instance, a young lady told me that in a large and crowded party
she caught her hair so firmly on the button of a passing servant, that it took some time before she could be
extricated; from her sensations she imagined that she had blushed crimson; but was assured by a friend that
she had turned extremely pale.
[4] Lieber `On the Vocal Sounds,' Smithsonian Contributions, 1851, vol. ii. p. 6.
[5] Ibid. p. 182.
I was desirous to learn how far down the body blushes extend; and Sir J. Paget, who necessarily has frequent
opportunities for observation, has kindly attended to this point for me during two or three years. He finds that
with women who blush intensely on the face, ears, and nape of neck, the blush does not commonly extend
any lower down the body. It is rare to see it as low down as the collarbones and shoulderblades; and he has
never himself seen a single instance in which it extended below the upper part of the chest. He has also
noticed that blushes sometimes die away downwards, not gradually and insensibly, but by irregular ruddy
blotches. Dr. Langstaff has likewise observed for me several women whose bodies did not in the least redden
while their faces were crimsoned with blushes. With. the insane, some of whom appear to be particularly
liable to blushing, Dr. J. Crichton Browne has several times seen the blush extend as far down as the
collarbones, and in two instances to the breasts. He gives me the case of a married woman, aged
twentyseven, who suffered from epilepsy. On the morning after her arrival in the Asylum, Dr. Browne,
together with his assistants, visited her whilst she was in bed. The moment that he approached, she blushed
deeply over her cheeks and temples; and the blush spread quickly to her ears. She was much agitated and
tremulous. He unfastened the collar of her chemise in order to examine the state of her lungs; and then a
brilliant blush rushed over her chest, in an arched line over the upper third of each breast, and extended
downwards between the breasts nearly to the ensiform cartilage of the sternum. This case is interesting, as the
blush did not thus extend downwards until it became intense by her attention being drawn to this part of her
person. As the examination proceeded she became composed, and the blush disappeared; but on several
subsequent occasions the same phenomena were observed.
[6] Moreau, in edit. of 1820 of Lavater, vol. iv. p. 303.
[7] Burgess. ibid. p. 38, on paleness after blushing, p. 177.
The foregoing facts show that, as a general rule, with English women, blushing does not extend beneath the
neck and upper part of the chest. Nevertheless Sir J. Paget informs me that he has lately heard of a case, on
which he can fully rely, in which a little girl, shocked by what she imagined to be an act of indelicacy,
blushed all over her abdomen and the upper parts of her legs. Moreau also[8] relates, on the authority of a
celebrated painter, that the chest, shoulders, arms, and whole body of a girl, who unwillingly consented to
serve as a model, reddened when she was first divested of her clothes.
It is a rather curious question why, in most cases the face, ears, and neck alone redden, inasmuch as the whole
surface of the body often tingles and grows hot. This seems to depend, chiefly, on the face and adjoining
parts of the skin having been habitually exposed to the air, light, and alternations of temperature, by which
the small arteries not only have acquired the habit of readily dilating and contracting, but appear to have
become unusually developed in comparison with other parts of the surface.[9] It is probably owing to this
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 121
Page No 124
same cause, as M. Moreau and Dr. Burgess have remarked, that the face is so liable to redden under various
circumstances, such as a feverfit. ordinary heat, violent exertion, anger, a slight blow, and on the other hand
that it is liable to grow pale from cold and fear, and to be discoloured during pregnancy. The face is also
particularly liable to be affected by cutaneous complaints, by smallpox, erysipelas, This view is likewise
supported by the fact that the men of certain races, who habitually go nearly naked, often blush over their
arms and chests and even down to their waists. A lady, who is a great blusher, informs Dr. Crichton Browne,
that when she feels ashamed or is agitated, she blushes over her face, neck, wrists, and hands,that is, over
all the exposed portions of her skin. Nevertheless it may be doubted whether the habitual exposure of the skin
of the face and neck, and its consequent power of reaction under stimulants of all kinds, is by itself sufficient
to account for the much greater tendency in English women of these parts than of others to blush; for the
hands are well supplied with nerves and small vessels, and have been as much exposed to the air as the face
or neck, and yet the hands rarely blush. We shall presently see that the attention of the mind having been
directed much more frequently and earnestly to the face than to any other part of the body, probably affords a
sufficient explanation.
[8] See Lavater, edit. of 1820, vol. iv. p. 303.
[9] Burgess, ibid. pp. 114, 122. Moreau in Lavater, ibid. vol. iv. p. 293.
_Blushing in the various races of man_.The small vessels of the face become filled with blood, from the
emotion of shame, in almost all the races of man, though in the very dark races no distinct change of colour
can be perceived. Blushing is evident in all the Aryan nations of Europe, and to a certain extent with those of
India. But Mr. Erskine has never noticed that the necks of the Hindoos are decidedly affected. With the
Lepchas of Sikhim, Mr. Scott has often observed a faint blush on the cheeks, base of the ears, and sides of the
neck, accompanied by sunken eyes and lowered head. This has occurred when he has detected them in a
falsehood, or has accused them of ingratitude. The pale, sallow complexions of these men render a blush
much more conspicuous than in most of the other natives of India. With the latter, shame, or it may be in part
fear, is expressed, according to Mr. Scott, much more plainly by the head being averted or bent down, with
the eyes wavering or turned askant, than by any change of colour in the skin.
The Semitic races blush freely, as might have been expected, from their general similitude to the Aryans.
Thus with the Jews, it is said in the Book of Jeremiah (chap. vi. 15), "Nay, they were not at all ashamed,
neither could they blush." Mrs. Asa Gray saw an Arab managing his boat clumsily on the Nile, and when
laughed at by his companions, "he blushed quite to the back of his neck." Lady Duff Gordon remarks that a
young Arab blushed on coming into her presence.[10]
Mr. Swinhoe has seen the Chinese blushing, but he thinks it is rare; yet they have the expression "to redden
with shame." Mr. Geach informs me that the Chinese settled in Malacca and the native Malays of the interior
both blush. Some of these people go nearly naked, and he particularly attended to the downward extension of
the blush. Omitting the cases in which the face alone was seen to blush, Mr. Geach observed that the face,
arms, and breast of a Chinaman, aged 24 years, reddened from shame; and with another Chinese, when asked
why he had not done his work in better style, the whole body was similarly affected. In two Malays[11] he
saw the face, neck, breast, and arms blushing; and in a third Malay (a Bugis) the blush extended down to the
waist.
The Polynesians blush freely. The Rev. Mr. Stack has seen hundreds of instances with the New Zealanders.
The following case is worth giving, as it relates to an old man who was unusually darkcoloured and partly
tattooed. After having let his land to an Englishman for a small yearly rental, a strong passion seized him to
buy a gig, which had lately become the fashion with the Maoris. He consequently wished to draw all the rent
for four years from his tenant, and consulted Mr. Stack whether he could do so. The man was old, clumsy,
poor, and ragged, and the idea of his driving himself about in his carriage for display amused Mr. Stack so
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 122
Page No 125
much that he could not help bursting out into a laugh; and then "the old man blushed up to the roots of his
hair." Forster says that "you may easily distinguish a spreading blush" on the cheeks of the fairest women in
Tahiti.[12] The natives also of several of the other archipelagoes in the Pacific have been seen to blush.
[10] `Letters from Egypt,' 1865, p. 66. Lady Gordon is mistaken when she says Malays and Mulattoes never
blush.
[11] Capt. Osborn (`Quedah,' p. 199), in speaking of a Malay, whom be reproached for cruelty, says he was
glad to see that the man blushed.
Mr. Washington Matthews has often seen a blush on the faces of the young squaws belonging to various wild
Indian tribes of North America. At the opposite extremity of the continent in Tierra del Fuego, the natives,
according to Mr. Bridges, "blush much, but chiefly in regard to women; but they certainly blush also at their
own personal appearance." This latter statement agrees with what I remember of the Fuegian, Jemmy Button,
who blushed when he was quizzed about the care which he took in polishing his shoes, and in otherwise
adorning himself. With respect to the Aymara Indians on the lofty plateaus of Bolivia, Mr. Forbes says,[13]
that from the colour of their skins it is impossible that their blushes should be as clearly visible as in the white
races; still under such circumstances as would raise a blush in us, "there can always be seen the same
expression of modesty or confusion; and even in the dark, a rise of temperature of the skin of the face can be
felt, exactly as occurs in the European." With the Indians who inhabit the hot, equable, and damp parts of
South America, the skin apparently does not answer to mental excitement so readily as with the natives of the
northern and southern parts of the continent, who have long been exposed to great vicissitudes of climate; for
Humboldt quotes without a protest the sneer of the Spaniard, "How can those be trusted, who know not how
to blush?"[14] Von Spix and Martius, in speaking of the aborigines of Brazil, assert that they cannot properly
be said to blush; "it was only after long intercourse with the whites, and after receiving some education, that
we perceived in the Indians a change of colour expressive of the emotions of their minds."[15] It is, however,
incredible that the power of blushing could have thus originated; but the habit of selfattention, consequent
on their education and new course of life, would have much increased any innate tendency to blush.
[12] J. R. Forster, `Observations during a Voyage round the World,' 4to, 1778, p. 229. Waitz gives
(`Introduction to Anthropology,' Eng. translat. 1863, vol. i. p. 135) references for other islands in the Pacific.
See, also, Dampier `On the Blushing of the Tunquinese' (vol. ii. p. 40); but I have not consulted this work.
Waitz quotes Bergmann, that the Kalmucks do not blush, but this may be doubted after what we have seen
with respect to the Chinese. He also quotes Roth, who denies that the Abyssinians are capable of blushing.
Unfortunately, Capt. Speedy, who lived so long with the Abyssinians, has not answered my inquiry on this
head. Lastly, I must add that the Rajah Brooke has never observed the least sign of a blush with the Dyaks of
Borneo; on the contrary under circumstances which would excite a blush in us, they assert "that they feel the
blood drawn from their faces."
[13] Transact. of the Ethnological Soc. 1870, vol. ii. p. 16.
Several trustworthy observers have assured me that they have seen on the faces of negroes an appearance
resembling a blush, under circumstances which would have excited one in us, though their skins were of an
ebonyblack tint. Some describe it as blushing brown, but most say that the blackness becomes more intense.
An increased supply of blood in the skin seems in some manner to increase its blackness; thus certain
exanthematous diseases cause the affected places in the negro to appear blacker, instead of, as with us,
redder.[16] The skin, perhaps, from being rendered more tense by the filling of the capillaries, would reflect a
somewhat different tint to what it did before. That the capillaries of the face in the negro become filled with
blood, under the emotion of shame, we may feel confident; because a perfectly characterized albino negress,
described by Buffon,[17] showed a faint tinge of crimson on her cheeks when she exhibited herself naked.
Cicatrices of the skin remain for a long time white in the negro, and Dr. Burgess, who had frequent
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 123
Page No 126
opportunities of observing a scar of this kind on the face of a negress, distinctly saw that it "invariably
became red whenever she was abruptly spoken to, or charged with any trivial offence."[18] The blush could
be seen proceeding from the circumference of the scar towards the middle, but it did not reach the centre.
Mulattoes are often great blushers, blush succeeding blush over their faces. From these facts there can be no
doubt that negroes blush, although no redness is visible on the skin.
[14] Humboldt, `Personal Narrative,' Eng. translat. vol. iii. p. 229.
[15] Quoted by Prichard, Phys. Hist. of Mankind, 4th edit 1851, vol. i. p. 271.
[16] See, on this head, Burgess, ibid. p. 32. Also Waitz, `Introdnction to Anthropology,' Eng. edit. vol. i. p.
139. Moreau gives a detailed account (`Lavater,' 1820, tom. iv. p. 302) of the blushing of a Madagascar
negressslave when forced by her brutal master to exhibit her naked bosom.
I am assured by Gaika and by Mrs. Barber that the Kafirs of South Africa never blush; but this may only
mean that no change of colour is distinguishable. Gaika adds that under the circumstances which would make
a, European blush, his countrymen "look ashamed to keep their heads up."
It is asserted by four of my informants that the Australians, who are almost as black as negroes, never blush.
A fifth answers doubtfully, remarking that only a very strong blush could be seen, on account of the dirty
state of their skins. Three observers state that they do blush;[19] Mr. S. Wilson adding that this is noticeable
only under a strong emotion, and when the skin is not too dark from long exposure and want of cleanliness.
Mr. Lang answers, "I have noticed that shame almost always excites a blush, which frequently extends as low
as the neck." Shame is also shown, as he adds, "by the eyes being turned from side to side." As Mr. Lang was
a teacher in a native school, it is probable that he chiefly observed children; and we know that they blush
more than adults. Mr. G. Taplin has seen halfcastes blushing, and he says that the aborigines have a word
expressive of shame. Mr. Hagenauer, who is one of those who has never observed the Australians to blush,
says that he has "seen them looking down to the ground on account of shame;" and the missionary, Mr.
Bulmer, remarks that though "I have not been able to detect anything like shame in the adult aborigines, I
have noticed that the eyes of the children, when ashamed, present a restless, watery appearance, as if they did
not know where to look."
[17] Quoted by Prichard, Phys. Hist. of Mankind, 4th edit. 1851, vol. i. p. 225.
[18] Burgess, ibid. p. 31. On mulattoes blushing, see p. 33. I have received similar accounts with respect to,
mulattoes.
[19] Barrington also says that the Australians of New South Wales blush, as quoted by Waitz, ibid. p. 135.
The facts now given are sufficient to show that blushing, whether or not there is any change of colour, is
common to most, probably to all, of the races of man.
_Movements and gestures which accompany Blushing_.Under a keen sense of shame there is a, strong
desire for concealment.[20] We turn away the whole body, more especially the face, which we endeavour in
some manner to hide. An ashamed person can hardly endure to meet the gaze of those present, so that he
almost invariably casts down his eyes or looks askant. As there generally exists at the same time a strong
wish to avoid the appearance of shame, a vain attempt is made to look direct at the person who causes this
feeling; and the antagonism between these opposite tendencies leads to various restless movements in the
eyes. I have noticed two ladies who, whilst blushing, to which they are very liable, have thus acquired, as it
appears, the oddest trick of incessantly blinking their eyelids with extraordinary rapidity. An intense blush is
sometimes accompanied by a slight effusion of tears;[21] and this, I presume, is due to the lacrymal glands
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 124
Page No 127
partaking of the increased supply of blood, which we know rushes into the capillaries of the adjoining parts,
including the retina.
[20] Mr. Wedgwood says (Dict. of English Etymology, vol. iii. 1865, p. 155) that the word shame "may well
originate in the idea of shade or concealment, and may be illustrated by the Low German _scheme_, shade or
shadow." Gratiolet (De la Phys. pp. 357362) has a good discussion on the gestures accompanying shame;
but some of his remarks seem to me rather fanciful. See, also, Burgess (ibid. pp. 69, 134) on the same subject.
Many writers, ancient and modern, have noticed the foregoing movements; and it has already been shown
that the aborigines in various parts of the world often exhibit their shame by looking downwards or askant, or
by restless movements of their eyes. Ezra cries out (ch. ix. 6), "O, my God! I am ashamed, and blush to lift up
my head to thee, my God." In Isaiah (ch. I. 6) we meet with the words, "I hid not my face from shame."
Seneca remarks (Epist. xi. 5) "that the Roman players hang down their heads, fix their eyes on the ground and
keep them lowered, but are unable to blush in acting shame." According to Macrobius, who lived in the filth
century (`Saturnalia,' B. vii. C. 11), "Natural philosophers assert that nature being moved by shame spreads
the blood before herself as a veil, as we see any one blushing often puts his hands before his face."
Shakspeare makes Marcus (`Titus Andronicus,' act ii, sc. 5) say to his niece, "Ah! now thou turn'st away thy
face for shame." A lady informs me that she found in the Lock Hospital a girl whom she had formerly known,
and who had become a wretched castaway, and the poor creature, when approached, hid her face under the
bedclothes, and could not be persuaded to uncover it. We often see little children, when shy or ashamed,
turn away, and still standing up, bury their faces in their mother's gown; or they throw themselves face
downwards on her lap.
[21] Burgess, ibid. pp. 181, 182. Boerhaave also noticed (as quoted by Gratiolet, ibid. p. 361) the tendency to
the secretion of tears during intense blushing. Mr. Bulmer, as we have seen, speaks of the "watery eyes" of
the children of the Australian aborigines when ashamed.
_Confusion of mind_.Most persons, whilst blushing intensely, have their mental powers confused. This is
recognized in such common expressions as "she was covered with confusion." Persons in this condition lose
their presence of mind, and utter singularly inappropriate remarks. They are often much distressed, stammer,
and make awkward movements or strange grimaces. In certain cases involuntary twitchings of some of the
facial muscles may be observed. I have been informed by a young lady, who blushes excessively, that at such
times she does not even know what she is saying. When it was suggested to her that this might be due to her
distress from the consciousness that her blushing was noticed, she answered that this could not be the case,
"as she had sometimes felt quite as stupid when blushing at a thought in her own room."
I will give an instance of the extreme disturbance of mind to which some sensitive men are liable. A
gentleman, on whom I can rely, assured me that he had been an eyewitness of the following scene:A
small dinnerparty was given in honour of an extremely shy man, who, when he rose to return thanks,
rehearsed the speech, which he had evidently learnt by heart, in absolute silence, and did not utter a single
word; but he acted as if he were speaking with much emphasis. His friends, perceiving how the case stood,
loudly applauded the imaginary bursts of eloquence, whenever his gestures indicated a pause, and the man
never discovered that he had remained the whole time completely silent. On the contrary, he afterwards
remarked to my friend, with much satisfaction, that he thought he had succeeded uncommonly well.
When a person is much ashamed or very shy, and blushes intensely, his heart beats rapidly and his breathing
is disturbed. This can hardly fail to affect the circulation of the blood within the brain, and perhaps the mental
powers. It seems however doubtful, judging from the still more powerful influence of anger and fear on the
circulation, whether we can thus satisfactorily account for the confused state of mind in persons whilst
blushing intensely.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 125
Page No 128
The true explanation apparently lies in the intimate sympathy which exists between the capillary circulation
of the surface of the head and face, and that of the brain. On applying to Dr. J. Crichton Browne for
information, he has given me various facts bearing on this subject. When the sympathetic nerve is divided on
one side of the head, the capillaries on this side are relaxed and become filled with blood, causing the skin to
redden and to grow hot, and at the same time the temperature within the cranium on the same side rises.
Inflammation of the membranes of the brain leads to the engorgement of the face, ears, and eyes with blood.
The first stage of an epileptic fit appears to be the contraction of the vessels of the brain, and the first outward
manifestation is, an extreme pallor of countenance. Erysipelas of the head commonly induces delirium. Even
the relief given to a severe headache by burning the skin with strong lotion, depends, I presume, on the same
principle.
Dr. Browne has often administered to his patients the vapour of the nitrite of amyl,[22] which has the singular
property of causing vivid redness of the face in from thirty to sixty seconds. This flushing resembles blushing
in almost every detail: it begins at several distinct points on the face, and spreads till it involves the whole
surface of the head, neck, and front of the chest; but has been observed to extend only in one case to the
abdomen. The arteries in the retina become enlarged; the eyes glisten, and in one instance there was a slight
effusion of tears. The patients are at first pleasantly stimulated, but, as the flushing increases, they become
confused and bewildered. One woman to whom the vapour had often been administered asserted that, as soon
as she grew hot, she grew MUDDLED. With persons just commencing to blush it appears, judging from their
bright eyes and lively behaviour, that their mental powers are somewhat stimulated. It is only when the
blushing is excessive that the mind grows confused. Therefore it would seem that the capillaries of the face
are affected, both during the inhalation of the nitrite of amyl and during blushing, before that part of the brain
is affected on which the mental powers depend.
Conversely when the brain is primarily affected; the circulation of the skin is so in a secondary manner. Dr.
Browne has frequently observed, as he informs me, scattered red blotches and mottlings on the chests of
epileptic patients. In these cases, when the skin on the thorax or abdomen is gently rubbed with a pencil or
other object, or, in stronglymarked cases, is merely touched by the finger, the surface becomes suffused in
less than half a minute with bright red marks, which spread to some distance on each side of the touched
point, and persist for several minutes. These are the _cerebral maculae_ of Trousseau; and they indicate, as
Dr. Browne remarks, a highly modified condition of the cutaneous vascular system. If, then, there exists, as
cannot be doubted, an intimate sympathy between the capillary circulation in that part of the brain on which
our mental powers depend, and in the skin of the face, it is not surprising that the moral causes which induce
intense blushing should likewise induce, independently of their own disturbing influence, much confusion of
mind.
[22] See also Dr. J. Crichton Browne's Memoir on this subject in the `West Riding Lunatic Asylum Medical
Report,' 1871, pp. 9598.
_The Nature of the Mental States which induce Blushing_.These consist of shyness, shame, and modesty;
the essential element in all being selfattention. Many reasons can be assigned for believing that originally
selfattention directed to personal appearance, in relation to the opinion of others, was the exciting cause; the
same effect being subsequently produced, through the force of association, by selfattention in relation to
moral conduct. It is not the simple act of reflecting on our own appearance, but the thinking what others think
of us, which excites a blush. In absolute solitude the most sensitive person would be quite indifferent about
his appearance. We feel blame or disapprobation more acutely than approbation; and consequently
depreciatory remarks or ridicule, whether of our appearance or conduct, causes us to blush much more readily
than does praise. But undoubtedly praise and admiration are highly efficient: a pretty girl blushes when a man
gazes intently at her, though she may know perfectly well that he is not depreciating her. Many children, as
well as old and sensitive persons blush, when they are much praised. Hereafter the question will be discussed,
how it has arisen that the consciousness that others are attending to our personal appearance should have led
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 126
Page No 129
to the capillaries, especially those of the face, instantly becoming filled with blood.
My reasons for believing that attention directed to personal appearance, and not to moral conduct, has been
the fundamental element in the acquirement of the habit of blushing, will now be given. They are separately
light, but combined possess, as it appears to me, considerable weight. It is notorious that nothing makes a shy
person blush so much as any remark, however slight, on his personal appearance. One cannot notice even the
dress of a woman much given to blushing, wihout causing her face to crimson. It is sufficient to stare hard at
some persons to make them, as Coleridge remarks, blush,"account for that he who can."[23]
With the two albinos observed by Dr. Burgess,[24] "the slightest attempt to examine their peculiarities
invariably" caused them to blush deeply. Women are much more sensitive about their personal appearance
than men are, especially elderly women in comparison with elderly men, and they blush much more freely.
The young of both sexes are much more sensitive on this same head than the old, and they also blush much
more freely than the old. Children at a very early age do not blush; nor do they show those other signs of
selfconsciousness which generally accompany blushing; and it is one of their chief charms that they think
nothing about what others think of them. At this early age they will stare at a stranger with a fixed gaze and
unblinking eyes, as on an inanimate object, in a manner which we elders cannot imitate.
[23] In a discussion on socalled animal magnetism in `Table Talk,' vol. i.
[24] Ibid. p. 40.
It is plain to every one that young men and women are highly sensitive to the opinion of each other with
reference to their personal appearance; and they blush incomparably more in the presence of the opposite sex
than in that of their own.[25] A young man, not very liable to blush, will blush intensely at any slight ridicule
of his appearance from a girl whose judgment on any important subject lie would disregard. No happy pair of
young lovers, valuing each other's admiration and love more than anything else in the world, probably ever
courted each other without many a blush. Even the barbarians of Tierra del Fuego, according to Mr. Bridges,
blush "chiefly in regard to women, but certainly also at their own personal appearance."
Of all parts of the body, the face is most considered and regarded, as is natural from its being the chief seat of
expression and the source of the voice. It is also the chief seat of beauty and of ugliness, and throughout the
world is the most ornamented.[26] The face, therefore, will have been subjected during many generations to
much closer and more earnest selfattention than any other part of the body; and in accordance with the
principle here advanced we can understand why it should be the most liable to blush. Although exposure to
alternations of temperature, has probably much increased the power of dilatation and contraction in the
capillaries of the face and adjoining parts, yet this by itself will hardly account for these parts blushing much
more than the rest of the body; for it does not explain the fact of the hands rarely blushing. With Europeans
the whole body tingles slightly when the face blushes intensely; and with the races of men who habitually go
nearly naked, the blushes extend over a much larger surface than with us. These facts are, to a certain extent,
intelligible, as the selfattention of primeval man, as well as of the existing races which still go naked, will
not have been so exclusively confined to their faces, as is the case with the people who now go clothed.
[25] Mr. Bain (`The Emotions and the Will,' 1865, p. 65) remarks on "the shyness of manners which is
induced between the sexes .... from the influence of mutual regard, by the apprehension on either side of not
standing well with the other."
[26] See, for evidence on this subject, `The Descent of Man,' vol. ii. pp. 71, 341.
We have seen that in all parts of the world persons who feel shame for some moral delinquency, are apt to
avert, bend down, or hide their faces, independently of any thought about their personal appearance. The
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 127
Page No 130
object can hardly be to conceal their blushes, for the face is thus averted or hidden under circumstances which
exclude any desire to conceal shame, as when guilt is fully confessed and repented of. It is, however,
probable that primeval man before he had acquired much moral sensitiveness would have been highly
sensitive about his personal appearance, at least in reference to the other sex, and he would consequently have
felt distress at any depreciatory remarks about his appearance; and this is one form of shame. And as the face
is the part of the body which is most regarded, it is intelligible that any one ashamed of his personal
appearance would desire to conceal this part of his body. The habit having been thus acquired, would
naturally be carried on when shame from strictly moral causes was felt; and it is not easy otherwise to see
why under these circumstances there should be a desire to hide the face more than any other part of the body.
The habit, so general with every one who feels ashamed, of turning away, or lowering his eyes, or restlessly
moving them from side to side, probably follows from each glance directed towards those present, bringing
home the conviction that he is intently regarded; and he endeavours, by not looking at those present, and
especially not at their eyes, momentarily to escape from this painful conviction.
_Shyness_.This odd state of mind, often called shamefacedness, or false shame, or _mauvaise honte_,
appears to be one of the most efficient of all the causes of blushing. Shyness is, indeed, chiefly recognized by
the face reddening, by the eyes being averted or cast down, and by awkward, nervous movements of the
body. Many a woman blushes from this cause, a hundred, perhaps a thousand times, to once that she blushes
from having done anything deserving blame, and of which she is truly ashamed. Shyness seems to depend on
sensitiveness to the opinion, whether good or bad, of others, more especially with respect to external
appearance. Strangers neither know nor care anything about our conduct or character, but they may, and often
do, criticize our appearance: hence shy persons are particularly apt to be shy and to blush in the presence of
strangers. The consciousness of anything peculiar, or even new, in the dress, or any slight blemish on the
person, and more especially, on the face points which are likely to attract the attention of strangers
makes the shy intolerably shy. On the other hand, in those cases in which conduct and not personal
appearance is concerned, we are much more apt to be shy in the presence of acquaintances, whose judgment
we in some degree value, than in that of strangers. A physician told me that a young man, a wealthy duke,
with whom he had travelled as medical attendant, blushed like a girl, when he paid him his fee; yet this young
man probably would not have blushed and been shy, had he been paying a bill to a tradesman. Some persons,
however, are so sensitive, that the mere act of speaking to almost any one is sufficient to rouse their
selfconsciousness, and a slight blush is the result.
Disapprobation or ridicule, from our sensitiveness on this head, causes shyness and blushing much more
readily than does approbation; though the latter with some persons is highly efficient. The conceited are
rarely shy; for they value themselves much too highly to expect depreciation. Why a proud man is often shy,
as appears to be the case, is not so obvious, unless it be that, with all his selfreliance, he really thinks much
about the opinion of others although in a disdainful spirit. Persons who are exceedingly shy are rarely shy in
the presence of those with whom they are quite familiar, and of whose good opinion and sympathy they are
perfectly assured; for instance, a girl in the presence of her mother. I neglected to inquire in my printed
paper whether shyness can be detected in the different races of man; but a Hindoo gentleman assured Mr.
Erskine that it is recognizable in his countrymen.
Shyness, as the derivation of the word indicates in several languages,[27] is closely related to fear; yet it is
distinct from fear in the ordinary sense. A shy man no doubt dreads the notice of strangers, but can hardly be
said to be afraid of them, he may be as bold as a hero in battle, and yet have no selfconfidence about trifles
in the presence of strangers. Almost every one is extremely nervous when first addressing a public assembly,
and most men remain so throughout their lives; but this appears to depend on the consciousness of a great
coming exertion, with its associated effects on the system, rather than on shyness;[28] although a timid or shy
man no doubt suffers on such occasions infinitely more than another. With very young children it is difficult
to distinguish between fear and shyness; but this latter feeling with them has often seemed to me to partake of
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 128
Page No 131
the character of the wildness of an untamed animal. Shyness comes on at a very early age. In one of my own
children, when two years and three months old, I saw a trace of what certainly appeared to be shyness,
directed towards myself after an absence from home of only a week. This was shown not by a blush, but by
the eyes being for a few minutes slightly averted from me. I have noticed on other occasions that shyness or
shamefacedness and real shame are exhibited in the eyes of young children before they have acquired the
power of blushing.
[27] H. Wedgwood, Dict. English Etymology, vol. iii. 1865, p. 184. So with the Latin word _verecundus_.
As shyness apparently depends on selfattention, we can perceive how right are those who maintain that
reprehending children for shyness, instead of doing them any good, does much harm, as it calls their attention
still more closely to themselves. It has been well urged that "nothing hurts young people more than to be
watched continually about their feelings, to have their countenances scrutinized, and the degrees of their
sensibility measured by the surveying eye of the unmerciful spectator. Under the constraint of such
examinations they can think of nothing but that they are looked at, and feel nothing but shame or
apprehension."[29]
[28] Mr. Bain (`The Emotions and the Will,' p. 64) has discussed the "abashed" feelings experienced on these
occasions, as well as the _stagefright_ of actors unused to the stage. Mr. Bain apparently attributes these
feelings to simple apprehension or dread.
[29] `Essays on Practical Education,' by Maria and R. L. Edgeworth, new edit. vol. ii. 1822, p. 38. Dr.
Burgess (ibid. p. 187) insists strongly to the same effect.
_Moral causes: guilt_.With respect to blushing from strictly moral causes, we meet with the same
fundamental principle as before, namely, regard for the opinion of others. It is not the conscience which
raises a blush, for a man may sincerely regret some slight fault committed in solitude, or he may suffer the
deepest remorse for an undetected crime, but he will not blush. "I blush," says Dr. Burgess,[30] "in the
presence of my accusers." It is not the sense of guilt, but the thought that others think or know us to be guilty
which crimsons the face. A man may feel thoroughly ashamed at having told a small falsehood, without
blushing; but if he even suspects that he is detected he will instantly blush, especially if detected by one
whom he reveres.
On the other hand, a man may be convinced that God witnesses all his actions, and he may feel deeply
conscious of some fault and pray for forgiveness; but this will not, as a lady who is a great blusher believes,
ever excite a blush. The explanation of this difference between the knowledge by God and man of our actions
lies, I presume, in man's disapprobation of immoral conduct being somewhat akin in nature to his
depreciation of our personal appearance, so that through association both lead to similar results; whereas the
disapprobation of God brings up no such association.
Many a person has blushed intensely when accused of some crime, though completely innocent of it. Even
the thought, as the lady before referred to has observed to me, that others think that we have made an unkind
or stupid remark, is amply sufficient to cause a blush, although we know all the time that we have been
completely misunderstood. An action may be meritorious or of an indifferent nature, but a sensitive person, if
he suspects that others take a different view of it, will blush. For instance, a lady by herself may give money
to a beggar without a trace of a blush, but if others are present, and she doubts whether they approve, or
suspects that they think her influenced by display, she will blush. So it will be, if she offers to relieve the
distress of a decayed gentlewoman, more particularly of one whom she had previously known under better
circumstances, as she cannot then feel sure how her conduct will be viewed. But such cases as these blend
into shyness.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 129
Page No 132
[29{sic, should be 30}] `Essays on Practical Education,' by Maria and R. L. Edgeworth, new edit. vol. ii.
1822, p. 50.
_Breaches of etiquette_.The rules of _etiquette_ always refer to conduct in the presence of, or towards
others. They have no necessary connection with the moral sense, and are often meaningless. Nevertheless as
they depend on the fixed custom of our equals and superiors, whose opinion we highly regard, they are
considered almost as binding as are the laws of honour to a gentleman. Consequently the breach of the laws
of etiquette, that is, any impoliteness or _gaucherie_, any impropriety, or an inappropriate remark, though
quite accidental, will cause the most intense blushing of which a man is capable. Even the recollection of
such an act, after an interval of many years, will make the whole body to tingle. So strong, also, is the power
of sympathy that a sensitive person, as a lady has assured me, will sometimes blush at a flagrant breach of
etiquette by a perfect stranger, though the act may in no way concern her.
_Modesty_.This is another powerful agent in exciting blushes; but the word modesty includes very
different states of the mind. It implies humility, and we often judge of this by persons being greatly pleased
and blushing at slight praise, or by being annoyed at praise which seems to them too high according to their
own humble standard of themselves. Blushing here has the usual signification of regard for the opinion of
others. But modesty frequently relates to acts of indelicacy; and indelicacy is an affair of etiquette, as we
clearly see with the nations that go altogether or nearly naked. He who is modest, and blushes easily at acts of
this nature, does so because they are breaches of a firmly and wisely established etiquette. This is indeed
shown by the derivation of the word _modest_ from _modus_, a measure or standard of behaviour. A blush
due to this form of modesty is, moreover, apt to be intense, because it generally relates to the opposite sex;
and we have seen how in all cases our liability to blush is thus increased. We apply the term `modest,' as it
would appear, to those who have an humble opinion of themselves, and to those who are extremely sensitive
about an indelicate word or deed, simply because in both cases blushes are readily excited, for these two
frames of mind have nothing else in common. Shyness also, from this same cause, is often mistaken for
modesty in the sense of humility.
Some persons flush up, as I have observed and have been assured, at any sudden and disagreeable
recollection. The commonest cause seems to be the sudden remembrance of not having done something for
another person which had been promised. In this case it may be that the thought passes half unconsciously
through the mind, "What will he think of me?" and then the flush would partake of the nature of a true blush.
But whether such flushes are in most cases due to the capillary circulation being affected, is very doubtful;
for we must remember that almost every strong emotion, such as anger or great joy, acts on the heart, and
causes the face to redden.
The fact that blushes may be excited in absolute solitude seems opposed to the view here taken, namely that
the habit originally arose from thinking about what others think of us. Several ladies, who are great blushers,
are unanimous in regard to solitude; and some of them believe that they have blushed in the dark. From what
Mr. Forbes has stated with respect to the Aymaras, and from my own sensations, I have no doubt that this
latter statement is correct. Shakspeare, therefore, erred when he made Juliet, who was not even by herself, say
to Romeo (act ii. sc. 2):
Thou know'st the mask of night is on my face;
Else would a maiden blush bepaint my cheek,
For that which thou hast heard me speak tonight."
But when a blush is excited in solitude, the cause almost always relates to the thoughts of others about usto
acts done in their presence, or suspected by them; or again when we reflect what others would have thought
of us had they known of the act. Nevertheless one or two of my informants believe that they have blushed
from shame at acts in no way relating to others. If this be so, we must attribute the result to the force of
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 130
Page No 133
inveterate habit and association, under a state of mind closely analogous to that which ordinarily excites a
blush; nor need we feel surprise at this, as even sympathy with another person who commits a flagrant breach
of etiquette is believed, as we have just seen, sometimes to cause a blush.
Finally, then, I conclude that blushing,whether due to shyness to shame for a real crimeto shame
from a breach of the laws of etiquetteto modesty from humilityto modesty from an
indelicacydepends in all cases on the same principle; this principle being a sensitive regard for the opinion,
more particularly for the depreciation of others, primarily in relation to our personal appearance, especially of
our faces; and secondarily, through the force of association and habit, in relation to the opinion of others on
our conduct.
_Theory of Blushing_.We have now to consider, why should the thought that others are thinking about us
affect our capillary circulation? Sir C. Bell insists[31] that blushing "is a provision for expression, as may be
inferred from the colour extending only to the surface of the face, neck, and breast, the parts most exposed. It
is not acquired; it is from the beginning." Dr. Burgess believes that it was designed by the Creator in "order
that the soul might have sovereign power of displaying in the cheeks the various internal emotions of the
moral feelings;" so as to serve as a check on ourselves, and as a sign to others, that we were violating rules
which ought to be held sacred. Gratiolet merely remarks,"Or, comme il est dans l'ordre de la nature que
l'etre social le plus intelligent soit aussi le plus intelligible, cette faculte de rougeur et de paleur qui distingue
l'homme, est un signe naturel de sa haute perfection."
The belief that blushing was SPECIALLY designed by the Creator is opposed to the general theory of
evolution, which is now so largely accepted; but it forms no part of my duty here to argue on the general
question. Those who believe in design, will find it difficult to account for shyness being the most frequent
and efficient of all the causes of blushing, as it makes the blusher to suffer and the beholder uncomfortable,
without being of the least service to either of them. They will also find it difficult to account for negroes and
other darkcoloured races blushing, in whom a change of colour in the skin is scarcely or not at all visible.
[31] Bell, `Anatomy of Expression,' p. 95. Burgess, as quoted below, ibid. p. 49. Gratiolet, De la Phys. p. 94.
No doubt a slight blush adds to the beauty of a maiden's face; and the Circassian women who are capable of
blushing, invariably fetch a higher price in the seraolio of the Sultan than less susceptible women.[32] But the
firmest believer in the efficacy of sexual selection will hardly suppose that blushing was acquired as a sexual
ornament. This view would also be opposed to what has. just been said about the darkcoloured races
blushing in an invisible manner.
The hypothesis which appears to me the most probable, though it may at first seem rash, is that attention
closely directed to any part of the body tends to interfere with the ordinary and tonic contraction of the small
arteries of that part. These vessels, in consequence, become at such times more or less relaxed, and are
instantly filled with arterial blood. This tendency will have been much strengthened, if frequent attention has
been paid during many generations to the same part, owing to nerveforce readily flowing along accustomed
channels, and by the power of inheritance. Whenever we believe that others are depreciating or even
considering our personal appearance, our attention is vividly directed to the outer and visible parts of our
bodies; and of all such parts we are most sensitive about our faces, as no doubt has been the case during many
past generations. Therefore, assuming for the moment that the capillary vessels can be acted on by close
attention, those of the face will have become eminently susceptible. Through the force of association, the
same effects will tend to follow whenever we think that others are considering or censuring our actions or
character.
As the basis of this theory rests on mental attention having some power to influence the capillary circulation,
it will be necessary to give a considerable body of details, bearing more or less directly on this subject.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 131
Page No 134
Several observers,[33] who from their wide experience and knowledge are eminently capable of forming a
sound judgment, are convinced that attention or consciousness (which latter term Sir H. Holland thinks the
more explicit) concentrated on almost any part of the body produces some direct physical effect on it. This
applies to the movements of the involuntary muscles, and of the voluntary muscles when acting
involuntarily, to the secretion of the glands,to the activity of the senses and sensations,and even to
the nutrition of parts.
[32] On the authority of Lady Mary Wortley Montague; see Burgess, ibid. p. 43.
It is known that the involuntary movements of the heart are affected if close attention be paid to them.
Gratiolet[34] gives the case of a man, who by continually watching and counting his own pulse, at last caused
one beat out of every six to intermit. On the other hand, my father told me of a careful observer, who
certainly had heartdisease and died from it, and who positively stated that his pulse was habitually irregular
to an extreme degree; yet to his great disappointment it invariably became regular as soon as my father
entered the room. Sir H. Holland remarks,[35] that "the effect upon the circulation of a part from the
consciousness suddenly directed and fixed upon it, is often obvious and immediate." Professor Laycock, who
has particularly attended to phenomena of this nature,[36] insists that "when the attention is directed to any
portion of the body, innervation and circulation are excited locally, and the functional activity of that portion
developed."
[33] In England, Sir H. Holland was, I believe, the first to consider the influence of mental attention on
various parts of the body, in his `Medical Notes and Reflections,' 1839 p. 64. This essay, much enlarged, was
reprinted by Sir H. Holland in his `Chapters on Mental Physiology,' 1858, p. 79, from which work I always
quote. At nearly the same time, as well as subsequently, Prof. Laycock discussed the same subject: see
`Edinburgh Medical and Surgical Journal,' 1839, July, pp. 1722. Also his `Treatise on the Nervous Diseases
of Women,' 1840, p. 110; and `Mind and Brain,' vol. ii. 1860, p. 327. Dr. Carpenter's views on mesmerism
have a nearly similar bearing. The great physiologist Muller treated (`Elements of Physiology,' Eng. translat.
vol. ii. pp. 937, 1085) of the influence of the attention on the senses. Sir J. Paget discusses the influence of
the mind on the nutrition of parts, in his `Lectures on Surgical Pathology,' 1853, vol. i. p. 39: 1 quote from the
3rd edit. revised by Prof. Turner, 1870, p. 28. See, also, Gratiolet, De la Phys. pp. 283287.
[34] De la Phys. p. 283.
It is generally believed that the peristaltic movements of the intestines are influenced by attention being paid
to them at fixed recurrent periods; and these movements depend on the contraction of unstriped and
involuntary muscles. The abnormal action of the voluntary muscles in epilepsy, chorea, and hysteria is known
to be influenced by the expectation of an attack, and by the sight of other patients similarly affected.[37] So it
is with the involuntary acts of yawning and laughing.
Certain glands are much influenced by thinking of them, or of the conditions under which they have been
habitually excited. This is familiar to every one in the increased flow of saliva, when the thought, for
instance, of intensely acid fruit is kept before the mind." It was shown in our sixth chapter, that an earnest and
longcontinued desire either to repress, or to increase, the action of the lacrymal glands is effectual. Some
curious cases have been recorded in the case of women, of the power of the mind on the mammary glands;
and still more remarkable ones in relation to the uterine functions.[39]
[35] `Chapters on Mental Physiology,' 1858, p. 111. [36] `Mind find Brain,' vol. ii. 1860, p. 327. [37]
`Chapters on Mental Physiology,' pp. 104106. [38] See Gratiolet on this subject, De la Phys. p. 287. [39] Dr.
J. Crichton Browne, from his observations on the insane, is convinced that attention directed for a prolonged
period on any part or organ may ultimately influence its capillary circulation and nutrition. He has given me
some extraordinary cases; one of these, which cannot here be related in full, refers to a married woman fifty
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 132
Page No 135
years of age, who laboured under the firm and longcontinued delusion that she was pregnant. When the
expected period arrived, she acted precisely as if she had been really delivered of a child, and seemed to
suffer extreme pain, so that the perspiration broke out on her forehead. The result was that a state of things
returned, continuing for three days, which had ceased during the six previous years. Mr. Braid gives, in his
`Magic, Hypnotism,' 1852, p. 95, and in his other works analogous cases, as well as other facts showing the
great influence of the will on the mammary glands, even on one breast alone.
When we direct our whole attention to any one sense, its acuteness is increased;[40] and the continued habit
of close attention, as with blind people to that of hearing, and with the blind and deaf to that of touch, appears
to improve the sense in question permanently. There is, also, some reason to believe, judging from the
capacities of different races of man, that the effects are inherited. Turning to ordinary sensations, it is well
known that pain is increased by attending to it; and Sir B. Brodie goes so far as to believe that pain may be
felt in any part of the body to which attention is closely drawn.[41] Sir H. Holland also remarks that we
become not only conscious of the existence of a part subjected to concentrated attention, but we experience in
it various odd sensations. as of weight, heat, cold, tingling, or itching.[42]
Lastly, some physiologists maintain that the mind can influence the nutrition of parts. Sir J. Paget has given a
curious instance of the power, not indeed of the mind, but of the nervous system, on the hair. A lady "who is
subject to attacks of what is called nervous headache, always finds in the morning after such an one, that
some patches of her hair are white, as if powdered with starch. The change is effected in a night, and in a few
days after, the hairs gradually regain their dark brownish colour.[43]
[40] Dr. Maudsley has given (`The Physiology and Pathology of Mind,' 2nd edit. 1868, p. 105), on good
authority, some curious statements with respect to the improvement of the sense of touch by practice and
attention. It is remarkable that when this sense has thus been rendered more acute at any point of the body,
for instance, in a finger, it is likewise improved at the corresponding point on the opposite side of the body.
[41] The Lancet,' 1838, pp. 3940, as quoted by Prof. Laycock, `Nervous Diseases of Women,' 1840, p. 110.
[42] `Chapters on Mental Physiology,' 1858, pp. 9193.
We thus see that close attention certainly affects various parts and organs, which are not properly under the
control of the will. By what means attentionperhaps the most wonderful of all the wondrous powers of the
mindis effected, is an extremely obscure subject. According to Muller,[44] the process by which the
sensory cells of the brain are rendered, through the will, susceptible of receiving more intense and distinct
impressions, is closely analogous to that by which the motor cells are excited to send nerveforce to the
voluntary muscles. There are many points of analogy in the action of the sensory and motor nervecells; for
instance, the familiar fact that close attention to any one sense causes fatigue, like the prolonged exertion of
any one muscle.[45] When therefore we voluntarily concentrate our attention on any part of the body, the
cells of the brain which receive impressions or sensations from that part are, it is probable, in some unknown
manner stimulated into activity. This may account, without any local change in the part to which our attention
is earnestly directed, for pain or odd sensations being there felt or increased.
[43] `Lectures on Surgical Pathology,' 3rd edit. revised by Prof. Turner, 1870, pp. 28, 31.
[44] `Elements of Physiology,' Eng. translat. vol. ii. p. 938.
[45] Prof. Laycock has discussed this point in a very interesting manner. See his `Nervous Diseases of
Women,' 1840, p. 110.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 133
Page No 136
If, however, the part is furnished with muscles, we cannot feel sure, as Mr. Michael Foster has remarked to
me, that some slight impulse may not be unconsciously sent to such muscles; and this would probably cause
an obscure sensation in the part.
In a large number of cases, as with the salivary and lacrymal glands, intestinal canal, the power of attention
seems to rest, either chiefly, or as some physiologists think, exclusively, on the vasomotor system being
affected in such a manner that more blood is allowed to flow into the capillaries of the part in question. This
increased action of the capillaries may in some cases be combined with the simultaneously increased activity
of the sensorium.
The manner in which the mind affects the vasomotor system may be conceived in the following manner.
When we actually taste sour fruit, an impression is sent through the gustatory nerves to a certain part of the
sensorium; this transmits nerveforce to the vasomotor centre, which consequently allows the muscular coats
of the small arteries that permeate the salivary glands to relax. Hence more blood flows into these glands, and
they secrete a copious supply of saliva. Now it does not seem an improbable assumption, that, when we
reflect intently on a sensation, the same part of the sensorium, or a closely connected part of it, is brought into
a state of activity, in the same manner as when we actually perceive the sensation. If so, the same cells in the
brain will be excited, though, perhaps, in a less degree, by vividly thinking about a sour taste, as by
perceiving it; and they will transmit in the one case, as in the other, nerveforce to the vasomotor centre
with the same results.
To give another, and, in some respects, more appropriate illustration. If a man stands before a hot fire, his
face reddens. This appears to be due, as Mr. Michael Foster informs me, in part to the local action of the heat,
and in part to a reflex action from the vasomotor centres.[46] In this latter case, the heat affects the nerves of
the face; these transmit an impression to the sensory cells of the brain, which act on the vasomotor centre,
and this reacts on the small arteries of the face, relaxing them and allowing them to become filled with blood.
Here, again, it seems not improbable that if we were repeatedly to concentrate with great earnestness our
attention on the recollection of our heated faces, the same part of the sensorium which gives us the
consciousness of actual heat would be in some slight degree stimulated, and would in consequence tend to
transmit some nerveforce to the vasomotor centres, so as to relax the capillaries of the face. Now as men
during endless generations have had their attention often and earnestly directed to their personal appearance,
and especially to their faces, any incipient tendency in the facial capillaries to be thus affected will have
become in the course of time greatly strengthened through the principles just referred to, namely, nerveforce
passing readily along accustomed channels, and inherited habit. Thus, as it appears to me, a plausible
explanation is afforded of the leading phenomena connected with the act of blushing.
_Recapitulation_.Men and women, and especially the young, have always valued, in a high degree, their
personal appearance; and have likewise regarded the appearance of others. The face has been the chief object
of attention, though, when man aboriginally went naked, the whole surface of his body would have been
attended to. Our selfattention is excited almost exclusively by the opinion of others, for no person living in
absolute solitude would care about his appearance. Every one feels blame more acutely than praise. Now,
whenever we know, or suppose, that others are depreciating our personal appearance, our attention is strongly
drawn towards ourselves, more especially to our faces. The probable effect of this will be, as has just been
explained, to excite into activity that part of the sensorium, which receives the sensory nerves of the face; and
this will react through the vasomotor system on the facial capillaries. By frequent reiteration during
numberless generations, the process will have become so habitual, in association with the belief that others
are thinking of us, that even a suspicion of their depreciation suffices to relax the capillaries, without any
conscious thought about our faces. With some sensitive persons it is enough even to notice their dress to
produce the same effect. Through the force, also, of association and inheritance our capillaries are relaxed,
whenever we know, or imagine, that any one is blaming, though in silence, our actions, thoughts, or
character; and, again, when we are highly praised.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIII. SELFATTENTIONSHAMESHYNESSMODESTY: BLUSHING. 134
Page No 137
[46] See, also, Mr. Michael Foster, on the action of the vasomotor system, in his interesting Lecture before
the royal Institution, as translated in the `Revue des Cours Scientifiques,' Sept. 25, 1869, p. 683.
On this hypothesis we can understand how it is that the face blushes much more than any other part of the
body, though the whole surface is somewhat affected, more especially with the races which still go nearly
naked. It is not at all surprising that the darkcoloured races should blush, though no change of colour is
visible in their skins. From the principle of inheritance it is not surprising that persons born blind should
blush. We can understand why the young are much more affected than the old, and women more than men;
and why the opposite sexes especially excite each other's blushes. It becomes obvious why personal remarks
should be particularly liable to cause blushing, and why the most powerful of all the causes is shyness; for
shyness relates to the presence and opinion of others, and the shy are always more or less selfconscious.
With respect to real shame from moral delinquencies, we can perceive why it is not guilt, but the thought that
others think us guilty, which raises a blush. A man reflecting on a crime committed in solitude, and stung by
his conscience, does not blush; yet he will blush under the vivid recollection of a detected fault, or of one
committed in the presence of others, the degree of blushing being closely related to the feeling of regard for
those who have detected, witnessed, or suspected his fault. Breaches of conventional rules of conduct, if they
are rigidly insisted on by our equals or superiors, often cause more intense blushes even than a detected
crime, and an act which is really criminal, if not blamed by our equals, hardly raises a tinge of colour on our
cheeks. Modesty from humility, or from an indelicacy, excites a vivid blush, as both relate to the judgment or
fixed customs of others.
From the intimate sympathy which exists between the capillary circulation of the surface of the head and of
the brain, whenever there is intense blushing, there will be some, and often great, confusion of mind. This is
frequently accompanied by awkward movements, and sometimes by the involuntary twitching of certain
muscles.
As blushing, according to this hypothesis, is an indirect result of attention, originally directed to our personal
appearance, that is to the surface of the body, and more especially to the face, we can understand the meaning
of the gestures which accompany blushing throughout the world. These consist in hiding the face, or turning
it towards the ground, or to one side. The eyes are generally averted or are restless, for to look at the man who
causes us to feel shame or shyness, immediately brings home in an intolerable manner the consciousness that
his gaze is directed on us. Through the principle of associated habit, the same movements of the face and eyes
are practised, and can, indeed, hardly be avoided, whenever we know or believe that, others are blaming, or
too strongly praising, our moral conduct.
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY.
The three leading principles which have determined the chief movements of expressionTheir
inheritanceOn the part which the will and intention have played in the acquirement of various
expressions The instinctive recognition of expressionThe bearing of our subject on the specific unity of
the races of manOn the successive acquirement of various expressions by the progenitors of man The
importance of expressionConclusion.
I HAVE now described, to the best of my ability, the chief expressive actions in man, and in some few of the
lower animals. I have also attempted to explain the origin or development of these actions through the three
principles given in the first chapter. The first of these principles is, that movements which are serviceable in
gratifying some desire, or in relieving some sensation, if often repeated, become so habitual that they are
performed, whether or not of any service, whenever the same desire or sensation is felt, even in a very weak
degree.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 135
Page No 138
Our second principle is that of antithesis. The habit of voluntarily performing opposite movements under
opposite impulses has become firmly established in us by the practice of our whole lives. Hence, if certain
actions have been regularly performed, in accordance with our first principle, under a certain frame of mind,
there will be a strong and involuntary tendency to the performance of directly opposite actions, whether or
not these are of any use, under the excitement of an opposite frame of mind.
Our third principle is the direct action of the excited nervous system on the body, independently of the will,
and independently, in large part, of habit. Experience shows that nerveforce is generated and set free
whenever the cerebrospinal system is excited. The direction which this nerveforce follows is necessarily
determined by the lines of connection between the nervecells, with each other and with various parts of the
body. But the direction is likewise much influenced by habit; inasmuch as nerveforce passes readily along
accustomed channels.
The frantic and senseless actions of an enraged man may be attributed in part to the undirected flow of
nerveforce, and in part to the effects of habit, for these actions often vaguely represent the act of striking.
They thus pass into gestures included under our first principle; as when an indignant man unconsciously
throws himself into a fitting attitude for attacking his opponent, though without any intention of making an
actual attack. We see also the influence of habit in all the emotions and sensations which are called exciting;
for they have assumed this character from having habitually led to energetic action; and action affects, in an
indirect manner, the respiratory and circulatory system; and the latter reacts on the brain. Whenever these
emotions or sensations are even slightly felt by us, though they may not at the time lead to any exertion, our
whole system is nevertheless disturbed through the force of habit and association. Other emotions and
sensations are called depressing, because they have not habitually led to energetic action, excepting just at
first, as in the case of extreme pain, fear, and grief, and they have ultimately caused complete exhaustion;
they are consequently expressed chiefly by negative signs and by prostration. Again, there are other
emotions, such as that of affection, which do not commonly lead to action of any kind, and consequently are
not exhibited by any strongly marked outward signs. Affection indeed, in as far as it is a pleasurable
sensation, excites the ordinary signs of pleasure.
On the other hand, many of the effects due to the excitement of the nervous system seem to be quite
independent of the flow of nerveforce along the channels which have been rendered habitual by former
exertions of the will. Such effects, which often reveal the state of mind of the person thus affected, cannot at
present be explained; for instance, the change of colour in the hair from extreme terror or grief, the cold
sweat and the trembling of the muscles from fear, the modified secretions of the intestinal canal,and the
failure of certain glands to act.
Notwithstanding that much remains unintelligible in our present subject, so many expressive movements and
actions can be explained to a certain extent through the above three principles, that we may hope hereafter to
see all explained by these or by closely analogous principles.
Actions of all kinds, if regularly accompanying any state of the mind, are at once recognized as expressive.
These may consist of movements of any part of the body, as the wagging of a dog's tail, the shrugging of a
man's shoulders, the erection of the hair, the exudation of perspiration, the state of the capillary circulation,
laboured breathing, and the use of the vocal or other soundproducing instruments. Even insects express
anger, terror, jealousy, and love by their stridulation. With man the respiratory organs are of especial
importance in expression, not only in a direct, but in a still higher degree in an indirect manner.
Few points are more interesting in our present subject than the extraordinarily complex chain of events which
lead to certain expressive movements. Take, for instance, the oblique eyebrows of a man suffering from grief
or anxiety. When infants scream loudly from hunger or pain, the circulation is affected, and the eyes tend to
become gorged with blood: consequently the muscles surrounding the eyes are strongly contracted as a
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 136
Page No 139
protection: this action, in the course of many generations, has become firmly fixed and inherited: but when,
with advancing years and culture, the habit of screaming is partially repressed, the muscles round the eyes
still tend to contract, whenever even slight distress is felt: of these muscles, the pyramidals of the nose are
less under the control of the will than are the others and their contraction can be checked only by that of the
central fasciae of the frontal muscle: these latter fasciae draw up the inner ends of the eyebrows, and wrinkle
the forehead in a peculiar manner, which we instantly recognize as the expression of grief or anxiety. Slight
movements, such as these just described, or the scarcely perceptible drawing down of the corners of the
mouth, are the last remnants or rudiments of strongly marked and intelligible movements. They are as full of
significance to us in regard to expression, as are ordinary rudiments to the naturalist in the classification and
genealogy of organic beings.
That the chief expressive actions, exhibited by man and by the lower animals, are now innate or
inherited,that is, have not been learnt by the individual,is admitted by every one. So little has learning or
imitation to do with several of them that they are from the earliest days and throughout life quite beyond our
control; for instance, the relaxation of the arteries of the skin in blushing, and the increased action of the heart
in anger. We may see children, only two or three years old, and even those born blind, blushing from shame;
and the naked scalp of a very young infant reddens from passion. Infants scream from pain directly after
birth, and all their features then assume the same form as during subsequent years. These facts alone suffice
to show that many of our most important expressions have not been learnt; but it is remarkable that some,
which are certainly innate, require practice in the individual, before they are performed in a full and perfect
manner; for instance, weeping and laughing. The inheritance of most of our expressive actions explains the
fact that those born blind display them, as I hear from the Rev. R. H. Blair, equally well with those gifted
with eyesight. We can thus also understand the fact that the young and the old of widely different races, both
with man and animals, express the same state of mind by the same movements.
We are so familiar with the fact of young and old animals displaying their feelings in the same manner, that
we hardly perceive how remarkable it is that a young puppy should wag its tail when pleased, depress its ears
and uncover its canine teeth when pretending to be savage, just like an old dog; or that a kitten should arch its
little back and erect its hair when frightened and angry, like an old cat. When, however, we turn to less
common gestures in ourselves, which we are accustomed to look at as artificial or conventional, such as
shrugging the shoulders, as a sign of impotence, or the raising the arms with open hands and extended
fingers, as a sign of wonder, we feel perhaps too much surprise at finding that they are innate. That these
and some other gestures are inherited, we may infer from their being performed by very young children, by
those born blind, and by the most widely distinct races of man. We should also bear in mind that new and
highly peculiar tricks, in association with certain states of the mind, are known to have arisen in certain
individuals, and to have been afterwards transmitted to their offspring, in some cases, for more than one
generation.
Certain other gestures, which seem to us so natural that we might easily imagine that they were innate,
apparently have been learnt like the words of a language. This seems to be the case with the joining of the
uplifted hands, and the turning up of the eyes, in prayer. So it is with kissing as a mark of affection; but this is
innate, in so far as it depends on the pleasure derived from contact with a beloved person. The evidence with
respect to the inheritance of nodding and shaking the head, as signs of affirmation and negation, is doubtful;
for they are not universal, yet seem too general to have been independently acquired by all the individuals of
so many races.
We will now consider how far the will and consciousness have come into play in the development of the
various movements of expression. As far as we can judge, only a few expressive movements, such as those
just referred to, are learnt by each individual; that is, were consciously and voluntarily performed during the
early years of life for some definite object, or in imitation of others, and then became habitual. The far greater
number of the movements of expression, and all the more important ones, are, as we have seen, innate or
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 137
Page No 140
inherited; and such cannot be said to depend on the will of the individual. Nevertheless, all those included
under our first principle were at first voluntarily performed for a definite object,namely, to escape some
danger, to relieve some distress, or to gratify some desire. For instance, there can hardly be a doubt that the
animals which fight with their teeth, have acquired the habit of drawing back their ears closely to their heads,
when feeling savage, from their progenitors having voluntarily acted in this manner in order to protect their
ears from being torn by their antagonists; for those animals which do not fight with their teeth do not thus
express a savage state of mind. We may infer as highly probable that we ourselves have acquired the habit of
contracting the muscles round the eyes, whilst crying gently, that is, without the utterance of any loud sound,
from our progenitors, especially during infancy, having experienced, during the act of screaming, an
uncomfortable sensation in their eyeballs. Again, some highly expressive movements result from the
endeavour to cheek or prevent other expressive movements; thus the obliquity of the eyebrows and the
drawing down of the corners of the mouth follow from the endeavour to prevent a screamingfit from
coming on, or to cheek it after it has come on. Here it is obvious that the consciousness and will must at first
have come into play; not that we are conscious in these or in other such cases what muscles are brought into
action, any more than when we perform the most ordinary voluntary movements.
With respect to the expressive movements due to the principle of antithesis, it is clear that the will has
intervened, though in a remote and indirect manner. So again with the movements coming under our third
principle; these, in as far as they are influenced by nerveforce readily passing along habitual channels, have
been determined by former and repeated exertions of the will. The effects indirectly due to this latter agency
are often combined in a complex manner, through the force of habit and association, with those directly
resulting from the excitement of the cerebrospinal system. This seems to be the case with the increased
action of the heart under the influence of any strong emotion. When an animal erects its hair, assumes a
threatening attitude, and utters fierce sounds, in order to terrify an enemy, we see a curious combination of
movements which were originally voluntary with those that are involuntary. It is, however, possible that even
strictly involuntary actions, such as the erection of the hair, may have been affected by the mysterious power
of the will.
Some expressive movements may have arisen spontaneously, in association with certain states of the mind,
like the tricks lately referred to, and afterwards been inherited. But I know of no evidence rendering this view
probable.
The power of communication between the members of the same tribe by means of language has been of
paramount importance in the development of man; and the force of language is much aided by the expressive
movements of the face and body. We perceive this at once when we converse on an important subject with
any person whose face is concealed. Nevertheless there are no grounds, as far as I can discover, for believing
that any muscle has been developed or even modified exclusively for the sake of expression. The vocal and
other soundproducing organs, by which various expressive noises are produced, seem to form a partial
exception; but I have elsewhere attempted to show that these organs were first developed for sexual purposes,
in order that one sex might call or charm the other. Nor can I discover grounds for believing that any
inherited movement, which now serves as a means of expression, was at first voluntarily and consciously
performed for this special purpose,like some of the gestures and the fingerlanguage used by the deaf and
dumb. On the contrary, every true or inherited movement of expression seems to have had some natural and
independent origin. But when once acquired, such movements may be voluntarily and consciously employed
as a means of communication. Even infants, if carefully attended to, find out at a very early age that their
screaming brings relief, and they soon voluntarily practise it. We may frequently see a person voluntarily
raising his eyebrows to express surprise, or smiling to express pretended satisfaction and acquiescence. A
man often wishes to make certain gestures conspicuous or demonstrative, and will raise his extended arms
with widely opened fingers above his head, to show astonishment, or lift his shoulders to his ears, to show
that he cannot or will not do something. The tendency to such movements will be strengthened or increased
by their being thus voluntarily and repeatedly performed; and the effects may be inherited.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 138
Page No 141
It is perhaps worth consideration whether movements at first used only by one or a few individuals to express
a certain state of mind may not sometimes have spread to others, and ultimately have become universal,
through the power of conscious and unconscious imitation. That there exists in man a strong tendency to
imitation, independently of the conscious will, is certain. This is exhibited in the most extraordinary manner
in certain brain diseases, especially at the commencement of inflammatory softening of the brain, and has
been called the "echo sign." Patients thus affected imitate, without understanding every absurd gesture which
is made, and every word which is uttered near them, even in a foreign language.[1] In the case of animals, the
jackal and wolf have learnt under confinement to imitate the barking of the dog. How the barking of the dog,
which serves to express various emotions and desires, and which is so remarkable from having been acquired
since the animal was domesticated, and from being inherited in different degrees by different breeds, was first
learnt we do not know; but may we not suspect that imitation has had something to do with its acquisition,
owing to dogs having long lived in strict association with so loquacious an animal as man?
[1] See the interesting facts given by Dr. Bateman on `Aphasia,' 1870, p. 110.
In the course of the foregoing remarks and throughout this volume, I have often felt much difficulty about the
proper application of the terms, will, consciousness, and intention. Actions, which were at first voluntary,
soon became habitual, and at last hereditary, and may then be performed even in opposition to the will.
Although they often reveal the state of the mind, this result was not at first either intended or expected. Even
such words as that "certain movements serve as a means of expression" are apt to mislead, as they imply that
this was their primary purpose or object. This, however, seems rarely or never to have been the case; the
movements having been at first either of some direct use, or the indirect effect of the excited state of the
sensorium. An infant may scream either intentionally or instinctively to show that it wants food; but it has no
wish or intention to draw its features into the peculiar form which so plainly indicates misery; yet some of the
most characteristic expressions exhibited by man are derived from the act of screaming, as has been
explained.
Although most of our expressive actions are innate or instinctive, as is admitted by everyone, it is a different
question whether we have any instinctive power of recognizing them. This has generally been assumed to be
the case; but the assumption has been strongly controverted by M. Lemoine.[2] Monkeys soon learn to
distinguish, not only the tones of voice of their masters, but the expression of their faces, as is asserted by a
careful observer.[3] Dogs well know the difference between caressing and threatening gestures or tones; and
they seem to recognize a compassionate tone. But as far as I can make out, after repeated trials, they do not
understand any movement confined to the features, excepting a smile or laugh; and this they appear, at least
in some cases, to recognize. This limited amount of knowledge has probably been gained, both by monkeys
and dogs, through their associating harsh or kind treatment with our actions; and the knowledge certainly is
not instinctive. Children, no doubt, would soon learn the movements of expression in their elders in the same
manner as animals learn those of man. Moreover, when a child cries or laughs, he knows in a general manner
what he is doing and what he feels; so that a very small exertion of reason would tell him what crying or
laughing meant in others. But the question is, do our children acquire their knowledge of expression solely by
experience through the power of association and reason?
As most of the movements of expression must have been gradually acquired, afterwards becoming
instinctive, there seems to be some degree of _a priori_ probability that their recognition would likewise have
become instinctive. There is, at least, no greater difficulty in believing this than in admitting that, when a
female quadruped first bears young, she knows the cry of distress of her offspring, or than in admitting that
many animals instinctively recognize and fear their enemies; and of both these statements there can be no
reasonable doubt. It is however extremely difficult to prove that our children instinctively recognize any
expression. I attended to this point in my firstborn infant, who could not have learnt anything by associating
with other children, and I was convinced that he understood a smile and received pleasure from seeing one,
answering it by another, at much too early an age to have learnt anything by experience. When this child was
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 139
Page No 142
about four months old, I made in his presence many odd noises and strange grimaces, and tried to look
savage; but the noises, if not too loud, as well as the grimaces, were all taken as good jokes; and I attributed
this at the time to their being preceded or accompanied by smiles. When five months old, he seemed to
understand a compassionate, expression and tone of voice. When a few days over six months old, his nurse
pretended to cry, and I saw that his face instantly assumed a melancholy expression, with the corners of the
mouth strongly depressed; now this child could rarely have seen any other child crying, and never a
grownup person crying, and I should doubt whether at so early an age he could have reasoned on the
subject. Therefore it seems to me that an innate feeling must have told him that the pretended crying of his
nurse expressed grief; and this through the instinct of sympathy excited grief in him.
[2] `La Physionomie et la Parole,' 1865, pp. 103, 118.
[3] Rengger, `Naturgeschichte der Saugethiere von Paraguay,' 1830, s. 55.
M. Lemoine argues that, if man possessed an innate knowledge of expression, authors and artists would not
have found it so difficult, as is notoriously the case, to describe and depict the characteristic signs of each
particular state of mind. But this does not seem to me a valid argument. We may actually behold the
expression changing in an unmistakable manner in a man or animal, and yet be quite unable, as I know from
experience, to analyse the nature of the change. In the two photographs given by Duchenne of the same old
man (Plate III. figs. 5 and 6), almost every one recognized that the one represented a true, and the other a
false smile; but I have found it very difficult to decide in what the whole amount of difference consists. It has
often struck me as a curious fact that so many shades of expression are instantly recognized without any
conscious process of analysis on our part. No one, I believe, can clearly describe a sullen or sly expression;
yet many observers are unanimous that these expressions can be recognized in the various races of man.
Almost everyone to whom I showed Duchenne's photograph of the young man with oblique eyebrows (Plate
II. fig. 2) at once declared that it expressed grief or some such feeling; yet probably not one of these persons,
or one out of a thousand persons, could beforehand have told anything precise about the obliquity of the
eyebrows with their inner ends puckered, or about the rectangular furrows on the forehead. So it is with many
other expressions, of which I have had practical experience in the trouble requisite in instructing others what
points to observe. If, then, great ignorance of details does not prevent our recognizing with certainty and
promptitude various expressions, I do not see how this ignorance can be advanced as an argument that our
knowledge, though vague and general, is not innate.
I have endeavoured to show in considerable detail that all the chief expressions exhibited by man are the
same throughout the world. This fact is interesting, as it affords a new argument in favour of the several races
being descended from a single parentstock, which must have been almost completely human in structure,
and to a large extent in mind, before the period at which the races diverged from each other. No doubt similar
structures, adapted for the same purpose, have often been independently acquired through variation and
natural selection by distinct species; but this view will not explain close similarity between distinct species in
a multitude of unimportant details. Now if we bear in mind the numerous points of structure having no
relation to expression, in which all the races of man closely agree, and then add to them the numerous points,
some of the highest importance and many of the most trifling value, on which the movements of expression
directly or indirectly depend, it seems to me improbable in the highest degree that so much similarity, or
rather identity of structure, could have been acquired by independent means. Yet this must have been the case
if the races of man are descended from several aboriginally distinct species. It is far more probable that the
many points of close similarity in the various races are due to inheritance from a single parentform, which
had already assumed a human character.
It is a curious, though perhaps an idle speculation, how early in the long line of our progenitors the various
expressive movements, now exhibited by man, were successively acquired. The following remarks will at
least serve to recall some of the chief points discussed in this volume. We may confidently believe that
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 140
Page No 143
laughter, as a sign of pleasure or enjoyment, was practised by our progenitors long before they deserved to be
called human; for very many kinds of monkeys, when pleased, utter a reiterated sound, clearly analogous to
our laughter, often accompanied by vibratory movements of their jaws or lips, with the corners of the mouth
drawn backwards and upwards, by the wrinkling of the cheeks, and even by the brightening of the eyes.
We may likewise infer that fear was expressed from an extremely remote period, in almost the same manner
as it now is by man; namely, by trembling, the erection of the hair, cold perspiration, pallor, widely opened
eyes, the relaxation of most of the muscles, and by the whole body cowering downwards or held motionless.
Suffering, if great, will from the first have caused screams or groans to be uttered, the body to be contorted,
and the teeth to be ground together. But our progenitors will not have exhibited those highly expressive
movements of the features which accompany screaming and crying until their circulatory and respiratory
organs, and the muscles surrounding the eyes, had acquired their present structure. The shedding of tears
appears to have originated through reflex action from the spasmodic contraction of the eyelids, together
perhaps with the eyeballs becoming gorged with blood during the act of screaming. Therefore weeping
probably came on rather late in the line of our descent; and this conclusion agrees with the fact that our
nearest allies, the anthropomorphous apes, do not weep. But we must here exercise some caution, for as
certain monkeys, which are not closely related to man, weep, this habit might have been developed long ago
in a subbranch of the group from which man is derived. Our early progenitors, when suffering from grief or
anxiety, would not have made their eyebrows oblique, or have drawn down the corners of their mouth, until
they had acquired the habit of endeavouring to restrain their screams. The expression, therefore, of grief and
anxiety is eminently human.
Rage will have been expressed at a very early period by threatening or frantic gestures, by the reddening of
the skin, and by glaring eyes, but not by frowning. For the habit of frowning seems to have been acquired
chiefly from the corrugators being the first muscles to contract round the eyes, whenever during infancy pain,
anger, or distress is felt, and there consequently is a near approach to screaming; and partly from a frown
serving as a shade in difficult and intent vision. It seems probable that this shading action would not have
become habitual until man had assumed a completely upright position, for monkeys do not frown when
exposed to a glaring light. Our early progenitors, when enraged, would probably have exposed their teeth
more freely than does man, even when giving full vent to his rage, as with the insane. We may, also, feel
almost certain that they would have protruded their lips, when sulky or disappointed, in a greater degree than
is the case with our own children, or even with the children of existing savage races.
Our early progenitors, when indignant or moderately angry, would not have held their heads erect, opened
their chests, squared their shoulders, and clenched their fists, until they had acquired the ordinary carriage and
upright attitude of man, and had learnt to fight with their fists or clubs. Until this period had arrived the
antithetical gesture of shrugging the shoulders, as a sign of impotence or of patience, would not have been
developed. From the same reason astonishment would not then have been expressed by raising the arms with
open hands and extended fingers. Nor, judging from the actions of monkeys, would astonishment have been
exhibited by a widely opened mouth; but the eyes would have been opened and the eyebrows arched. Disgust
would have been shown at a very early period by movements round the mouth, like those of vomiting,that
is, if the view which I have suggested respecting the source of the expression is correct, namely, that our
progenitors had the power, and used it, of voluntarily and quickly rejecting any food from their stomachs
which they disliked. But the more refined manner of showing contempt or disdain, by lowering the eyelids, or
turning away the eyes and face, as if the despised person were not worth looking at, would not probably have
been acquired until a much later period.
Of all expressions, blushing seems to be the most strictly human; yet it is common to all or nearly all the
races of man, whether or not any change of colour is visible in their skin. The relaxation of the small arteries
of the surface, on which blushing depends, seems to have primarily resulted from earnest attention directed to
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 141
Page No 144
the appearance of our own persons, especially of our faces, aided by habit, inheritance, and the ready flow of
nerveforce along accustomed channels; and afterwards to have been extended by the power of association to
selfattention directed to moral conduct. It can hardly be doubted that many animals are capable of
appreciating beautiful colours and even forms, as is shown by the pains which the individuals of one sex take
in displaying their beauty before those of the opposite sex. But it does not seem possible that any animal,
until its mental powers had been developed to an equal or nearly equal degree with those of man, would have
closely considered and been sensitive about its own personal appearance. Therefore we may conclude that
blushing originated at a very late period in the long line of our descent.
From the various facts just alluded to, and given in the course of this volume, it follows that, if the structure
of our organs of respiration and circulation had differed in only a slight degree from the state in which they
now exist, most of our expressions would have been wonderfully different. A very slight change in the course
of the arteries and veins which run to the head, would probably have prevented the blood from accumulating
in our eyeballs during violent expiration; for this occurs in extremely few quadrupeds. In this case we should
not have displayed some of our most characteristic expressions. If man had breathed water by the aid of
external branchiae (though the idea is hardly conceivable), instead of air through his mouth and nostrils, his
features would not have expressed his feelings much more efficiently than now do his hands or limbs. Rage
and disgust, however, would still have been shown by movements about the lips and mouth, and the eyes
would have become brighter or duller according to the state of the circulation. If our ears had remained
movable, their movements would have been highly expressive, as is the case with all the animals which fight
with their teeth; and we may infer that our early progenitors thus fought, as we still uncover the canine tooth
on one side when we sneer at or defy any one, and we uncover all our teeth when furiously enraged.
The movements of expression in the face and body, whatever their origin may have been, are in themselves
of much importance for our welfare. They serve as the first means of communication between the mother and
her infant; she smiles approval, and thus encourages her child on the right path, or frowns disapproval. We
readily perceive sympathy in others by their expression; our sufferings are thus mitigated and our pleasures
increased; and mutual good feeling is thus strengthened. The movements of expression give vividness and
energy to our spoken words. They reveal the thoughts and intentions of others more truly than do words,
which may be falsified. Whatever amount of truth the socalled science of physiognomy may contain,
appears to depend, as Haller long ago remarked,[4] on different persons bringing into frequent use different
facial muscles, according to their dispositions; the development of these muscles being perhaps thus
increased, and the lines or furrows on the face, due to their habitual contraction, being thus rendered deeper
and more conspicuous. The free expression by outward signs of an emotion intensifies it. On the other hand,
the repression, as far as this is possible, of all outward signs softens our emotions.[5] He who gives way to
violent gestures will increase his rage; he who does not control the signs of fear will experience fear in a
greater degree; and he who remains passive when overwhelmed with grief loses his best chance of recovering
elasticity of mind. These results follow partly from the intimate relation which exists between almost all the
emotions and their outward manifestations; and partly from the direct influence of exertion on the heart, and
consequently on the brain. Even the simulation of an emotion tends to arouse it in our minds. Shakespeare,
who from his wonderful knowledge of the human mind ought to be an excellent judge, says:
Is it not monstrous that this player here,
But in a fiction, in a dream of passion,
Could force his soul so to his own conceit,
That, from her working, all his visage wann'd;
Tears in his eyes, distraction in 's aspect,
A broken voice, and his whole function suiting
With forms to his conceit? And all for nothing!
_Hamlet_, act ii. sc. 2.
[4] Quoted by Moreau, in his edition of Lavater, 1820, tom. iv. p. 211.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 142
Page No 145
We have seen that the study of the theory of expression confirms to a certain limited extent the conclusion
that man is derived from some lower animal form, and supports the belief of the specific or subspecific
unity of the several races; but as far as my judgment serves, such confirmation was hardly needed. We have
also seen that expression in itself, or the language of the emotions, as it has sometimes been called, is
certainly of importance for the welfare of mankind. To understand, as far as possible, the source or origin of
the various expressions which may be hourly seen on the faces of the men around us, not to mention our
domesticated animals, ought to possess much interest for us. From these several causes, we may conclude
that the philosophy of our subject has well deserved the attention which it has already received from several
excellent observers, and that it deserves still further attention, especially from any able physiologist.
[5] Gratiolet (`De la Physionomie,' 1865, p. 66) insists on the truth of this conclusion.
The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY. 143
Bookmarks
1. Table of Contents, page = 3
2. The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals, page = 4
3. Charles Darwin, page = 4
4. INTRODUCTION., page = 4
5. CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION., page = 14
6. CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION--_continued_., page = 24
7. CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION--_concluded_., page = 28
8. CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS., page = 35
9. CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS., page = 47
10. CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING., page = 58
11. CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR., page = 70
12. CHAPTER VIII. Joy, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION., page = 78
13. CHAPTER IX. REFLECTION--MEDITATION-ILL-TEMPER--SULKINESS--DETERMINATION., page = 88
14. CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER., page = 94
15. CHAPTER XI. DISDAIN--CONTEMPT--DISGUST-GUILT--PRIDE, ETC.--HELPLESSNESS--PATIENCE-- AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION., page = 101
16. CHAPTER XII. SURPRISE--ASTONISHMENT--FEAR--HORROR., page = 111
17. CHAPTER XIII. SELF-ATTENTION--SHAME--SHYNESS--MODESTY: BLUSHING., page = 122
18. CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY., page = 138